Subscribe to RSS
DOI: 10.1055/a-2433-3930
Psychophysiological markers of athlete burnout: a call to arms
Abstract
Burnout is a growing problem in high-performance sport and has negative consequences for athletes (e.g., mental ill-health). It is therefore important to effectively monitor athlete burnout to aid intervention efforts. While self-report measures are available (e.g., athlete burnout questionnaire), the limitations associated with these measures (e.g., social desirability bias) means that objective physiological markers may also be useful. Thus, this article critically discusses potential biomarkers of athlete burnout, drawing on research inside and outside of sport to offer an overview of the current state-of-the-art in this research area. First, it outlines what athlete burnout is, its deleterious consequences, and discusses existing psychological assessments. The article then critically discusses literature on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (e.g., salivary cortisol) and autonomic nervous system (e.g., heart rate variability) indices of burnout, highlighting some promising biomarkers for future research (e.g., salivary cortisol at bedtime, vagally-mediated heart rate variability at rest). Finally, the article concludes by highlighting key considerations and offering recommendations for future research (e.g., use of more homogenous methods in assessing burnout and physiological parameters). As a result, the intention of this article is to spark more higher quality research on the psychophysiology of athlete burnout, thereby helping tackle this prominent issue in high-performance sport.
#
Introduction
Burnout is a multifaceted phenomenon that was first identified as a problem in caregivers (e. g., doctors, nurses) [1] [2]. In the 1970s, it was speculated that athletes may be susceptible to burnout [3], and these concerns grew in the mid-1980s and early 1990s [4] [5]. As research on athlete burnout accumulated, Raedeke [6] drew on the original work of Maslach and proposed a sport-specific conceptualization characterized by three dimensions: (1) physical and emotional exhaustion (i. e., perceived depletion of one’s emotional and physical resources resulting from training and competition), sport devaluation (i. e., negative feelings towards oneʼs sport), and reduced sense of accomplishment (i. e., a negative evaluation of one’s sporting abilities and achievements). Studies have shown that ~10% of athletes may be experiencing moderate to severe burnout at any given time [7] [8], and the prevalence of athlete burnout is on the rise [9]. This is worrying given that it has been linked with various deleterious outcomes (e. g., dropout from sport) [10]. For instance, in a recent meta-analysis, Glandorf et al. [11] found that higher athlete burnout was associated with poorer mental health (e. g., higher depression symptomology) and well-being (e. g., lower life satisfaction). Given that burnout is a growing problem that athletes face, and it has clear detrimental impacts on health and well-being, more emphasis is needed on accurate identification, prevention, and treatment [12]. To aid such efforts, it is crucial that valid and reliable burnout measures are developed.
Various questionnaires have been used to assess athlete burnout (e. g., Shirom Melamed Burnout Measure; SMBM) [13], with the most common being the Athlete Burnout Questionnaire (ABQ) [14]. Although the ABQ aligns with Raedeke’s [6] conceptualization and is recommended for use with athletes [15], it has been criticized [7]. For example, there are concerns about how it measures emotional and physical exhaustion [16], with some arguing that its items fail to directly assess either construct (e. g., “I feel wiped out from sport”). As a result, other tools have emerged tackling some of these issues (e. g., items from the Athlete Burnout Scale more directly assess physical exhaustion) [17]. Although self-report measures can help monitor athlete burnout, their utility in isolation may be limited. For instance, with assessments like the ABQ, no reliable cut-offs have been established, making it difficult to determine if an athlete is experiencing moderate to severe burnout symptoms and in need of intervention [7]. Moreover, athletes may find it challenging to admit feelings of exhaustion, negativity about their sport, and performance-related concerns due to fear of judgment (e. g., by coaches) [18]. This could lead athletes to conceal their true feelings and respond in a way that masks their vulnerabilities, introducing social desirability bias. Indeed, athlete burnout still carries some stigma [19], meaning that the accuracy of self-report data might be negatively impacted by bias (e. g., self-presentation) [20]. As such, practitioners (e. g., sports scientists) may struggle to rely solely on self-report data and might benefit from drawing on additional sources of information, such as objective measurements (e. g., biomarkers) [11].
At present, very little is known about the psychophysiology of athlete burnout, including its etiology and potential biomarkers (e. g., salivary cortisol) [15]. Thus, the aim of this article is to provide a novel summary and critique of existing literature on potential biomarkers of burnout (e. g., heart rate variability; HRV), largely drawing on research outside of sport to offer vital information that can aid future research and guide applied practice.
#
Psychophysiological markers of burnout
Hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis (HPA) measures
Most studies searching for a biomarker of burnout have used HPA axis measures [21]. Despite its involvement in other processes (e. g., fighting illness or infection), the HPA axis is best known as a stress regulation system; and when a stressor is perceived, it is activated by the limbic part of the brain (e. g., amygdala) and results in the sequential release of corticotropin hormone from the hypothalamus, adrenocorticotropic hormone from the pituitary gland, and cortisol from the adrenal cortex [22]. This release of hormones into the blood mobilizes substrates for energy metabolism, supplying the body with the energy required to actively cope with the stressor [23]. As the end-product of the HPA axis, most research has focused on cortisol, largely through saliva samples but less commonly via blood (serum), hair, or urine [24]. Cortisol levels often increase in response to a stressor [25] and tend to follow a diurnal pattern in which they steadily increase in the second half of the night, peak shortly after waking (i. e., within 30 to 60 minutes), and then gradually decrease across the day [26]. Due to this diurnal rhythm, and the fact that cortisol levels are affected by many factors (e. g., diet, sleep) [27], there are complexities in measuring it, and various approaches have been used including collecting single (e. g., pre-work) or multiple (e. g., cortisol awakening response) morning samples, and samples taken serially across the day (e. g., afternoon, evening) [21].
Research linking burnout with salivary cortisol assessed via one-off morning samples has revealed mixed results [22]. While some studies have shown null effects [e. g., 28] or a negative association [e. g., 29, 30], more than half of these studies found that burnout was associated with elevated morning cortisol [e. g., 31, 32, 33, 34]. For example, well-powered studies by Deneva et al. [33] and Wingenfeld et al. [34] found that burnt-out medical professionals (303 doctors and 279 nurses, respectively) had higher morning cortisol than their colleagues. However, in both studies, cortisol samples were taken at the start of a workday (i. e., at approximately 7 am) and thus did not reflect, or account for, waking levels or time. This makes the findings difficult to interpret given cortisol’s diurnal rhythm (e. g., the typical increase observed 30 to 60 minutes after waking) and that various uncaptured factors could have impacted cortisol concentrations in the time between waking and arriving at work (e. g., ambient light level, anticipation of the day ahead, caffeine consumption) [35]. Indeed, variations in sample timing and a lack of control for confounding factors likely explains the mixed findings across studies employing single morning samples. For instance, samples were reportedly taken at 7 AM in two studies [30] [34], 8 AM in two studies [28] [32], between 6 AM and 8 AM in one study [33], and at an unspecified time in two studies [29] [31]. Given these issues, expert consensus guidelines strongly advise against one-off morning samples, instead recommending multiple morning samples (e. g., to assess the cortisol awakening response) while controlling for crucial confounding factors like time of awakening [35].
The cortisol awakening response, defined as the rapid rise in cortisol levels that typically occurs in the morning after waking (usually within 30 to 60 minutes), has been the subject of much research and can be assessed in numerous ways (e. g., from area-under-the-curve metrics to change scores) [36]. The findings from research assessing burnout in relation to the cortisol awakening response have also been equivocal [36], with studies reporting null effects [e. g., 37], positive associations [e. g., 38], or negative associations [e. g., 39]. Indeed, these mixed findings are likely due, at least in part, to inconsistencies in how the awakening response has been assessed. For example, while Sjors and Jónsdóttir [39] utilized change scores (i. e., difference between awakening and 15 minutes later), McCanlies et al. [37] and Traunmuller et al. [38] used area-under-the-curve metrics (i. e., ground and increment) from a greater number of samples (e. g., awakening and 15, 30, and 45 minutes later). Despite the unclear results, it is worth noting that one of the more rigorous (e. g., confounding factors such as awakening time, cigarette smoking, drug use, etc. were controlled for in the analyses and/or protocol) and largest datasets collected to date by Marchand and colleagues [40] [41] [42] reported that higher burnout was related to a blunted cortisol awakening response (i. e., less change from waking to 30 minutes after waking) in a sample of 401 Canadian day-shift workers. Indeed, this finding mirrors the conclusion from an earlier meta-analysis by Chida and Steptoe [43], as well as sport-based research linking salivary cortisol to a concept related to burnout, overtraining syndrome [e. g., 44]. As such, the cortisol awakening response might be worth examining in future work alongside other potential biomarkers (e. g., diurnal cortisol).
Further to research centered on morning cortisol, studies have investigated if burnout impacts salivary cortisol secretion across the rest of the day (e. g., afternoon, evening) [21]. Overall, the findings on diurnal cortisol have been mixed, with some studies reporting null effects [e. g., 45], and others revealing a positive [e. g., 46] or negative [e. g., 47] association. Again, this is likely due, at least in part, to variations in how and when diurnal cortisol was assessed. For instance, studies differed in whether they analyzed diurnal cortisol via one-off samples (e. g., at the end of a work shift) [46], a single metric (e. g., diurnal slope from awakening to bedtime) [37], or multiple samples while factoring in morning cortisol levels (e. g., waking, 30 minutes later, 11 AM, 3 PM, and 8 PM) [45]. However, despite the equivocal results, it is worth noting that when focused solely on studies using non-clinical samples of a relatively large size (i. e., ≥ 200 participants) that measured burnout in a manner most consistent with athlete burnout (i. e., via Maslach Burnout Inventory; MBI) [48], a clearer picture emerges. Indeed, Marchand et al. [41] collected salivary cortisol from 401 Canadian workers at awakening, 30 minutes later, 2 PM, 4 PM, and bedtime, and found that higher burnout (and particularly exhaustion) was linked with lower cortisol in the afternoon and evening, with the largest effect at bedtime. Similarly, among 197 police officers, McCanlies et al. [37] found that greater exhaustion and cynicism were associated with less diurnal cortisol secretion, with samples collected at lunchtime, dinner, and bedtime. In contrast, Wingenfeld et al. [34] found that burnt-out nurses had higher salivary cortisol than colleagues across all samples (i. e., 11:30 AM, 5:30 PM, and 8 PM). This conflicting finding, which mirrors research linking hypercortisolism with depression – a condition thought to overlap with burnout [36] – could be due to methodological differences between studies (e. g., populations).
Many reviews published to date have concluded that there is no single compelling HPA axis-related marker of burnout, attributing this to the high methodological heterogeneity and weaknesses inherent in the literature (but it might be that burnout is not reflected in any HPA-axis measures) [21]. First, the way in which burnout has been conceptualized has varied between studies [36]. For example, while some studies have used Maslach’s [48] multidimensional definition (i. e., emotional exhaustion, cynicism, personal inefficacy) [49], others have explored burnout via the dimensions (e. g., cognitive weariness, emotional exhaustion, physical fatigue; [50]) developed by Shirom and Melamed [e. g., 51]. Second, studies have differed in how they have treated burnout data [52], either analyzing it in a continuous fashion [e. g., 41] or, most often, grouping participants using cut-points that differ from study-to-study [e. g., 53]. Third, the population has varied greatly between studies [24], ranging from non-clinical (e. g., teachers) to clinical (e. g., patients with exhaustion disorder; [54]) samples. Fourth, studies have differed in when biomarker data was collected (e. g., 15, 30, 45, or 60 minutes after waking) [36], and if they controlled statistically for confounding factors (e. g., age) [21]. Fifth, most studies have used small samples and cross-sectional designs, which limit statistical power and causal understanding [22]. Given these issues, future work evaluating HPA-axis related markers of burnout should strive for greater methodological homogeneity (e. g., in burnout questionnaire use) and employ stronger methods (e. g., longitudinal study designs). Unfortunately, the few studies that have been conducted in sport also suffer from the issues noted above (e. g., small sample sizes; see [Table 1]).
Authors (Year) |
Study Design |
Sample (n) |
BO Measure |
Sample Timing |
Key Results |
Effect Sizes |
Main Limitations |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Davis et al. (2018) |
Cross-sectional |
82 athletes (55 male, 27 female; M age =20 years) |
ABQ |
Salivary cortisol sampled at 7–9 am, 10–11 am, 1–3pm, or 6–8
pm based on training timing |
BO was not significantly associated with change in cortisol |
r=0.10 |
Small sample size |
Dobson et al. (2020) |
Repeated measures observational |
13 female swimmers (M age =19 years) |
ABQ at beginning of season, in period of overload training, and after a taper (11 weeks between each measure) |
Resting lnRMSSD assessed for 10 minutes in supine position at
three time points via Finapres (lnRMSSD from last 5 minutes
used in analyses) |
BO was highest in tapering period and lnRMSSD was lowest in
overload period |
Not available |
Small sample size |
Landolt et al. (2019) |
Cross-sectional |
32 professional jockeys (14 male, 18 female; M age =19 years) |
MBI |
Salivary cortisol sample at awakening and 30 minutes after in
low- and high-stress period of season |
No significant associations between BO dimensions and CAR
apart from positive relationship between cynicism and CAR in
high-stress period |
r between BO dimensions and CAR in low stress period
ranged from 0.06 to 0.27 |
Small sample size |
Martin et al. (2022) |
Longitudinal |
40 NCAA swimmers (17 male, 23 females; M age = 20 years) |
ABQ |
Serum cortisol at beginning and end of a 6-week training
period, no specific time of day stated |
BO was positively associated with change in the ratio of
testosterone to cortisol, which was mostly driven by a ↓ in
cortisol |
r=0.34 |
Small sample size |
Monfared et al. (2021) |
Cross-sectional |
42 youth athletes (14 males, 28 females; M age = 15 years) |
ABQ |
Salivary cortisol sample 30 minutes before and immediately
before practice, but no specific time of day
stated |
Significant positive association between BO and change in
cortisol |
β=0.24 |
Small sample size |
Souza et al. (2018) |
Case-control |
12 BO and 12 control soccer and futsal players (gender not stated but M age = 21 years) |
ABQ |
Salivary cortisol sample at 8 am |
No significant differences in salivary cortisol between
groups |
d=0.32 |
Small sample size |
Notes: ABQ, athlete burnout questionnaire; BO, burnout; BVP, blood volume pulse; CAR, cortisol awakening response; CK, creatine kinase; DHEA-S, dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate; GSR, galvanic skin response; HR, heart rate; lnRMSSD, natural log of RMSSD; MBI, Maslach burnout inventory; MG, myoglobin; NCAA, national collegiate athletic association; RMSSD, root mean square of successive differences of r-to-r intervals; RR, respiration rate; sAA-AR, Salivary alpha-amylase awakening responses.
Like the research noted outside of sport, the few studies conducted in sport have revealed mixed results. Indeed, most research has reported null effects between athlete burnout and cortisol [e. g., 55]. For instance, Souza et al. [28] split soccer players into burnt out and control groups based on ABQ scores and found no significant between-group differences in morning salivary cortisol (assessed via one sample at 8 AM). Similarly, among professional jockeys, Landolt et al. [56] found few significant associations between burnout dimensions assessed via the MBI and cortisol awakening response, with only a positive relationship emerging between cynicism and the cortisol awakening response during a high-stress period of the season. Monfared et al. [57] also reported a significant positive correlation, with higher burnout (measured via the ABQ) associated with greater increases in salivary cortisol from 30 minutes to immediately before training in 42 youth athletes. Finally, in a study with 40 collegiate-level swimmers, Martin et al. [58] collected serum cortisol samples at the beginning and end of a six-week training block and found that burnout was significantly and positively associated with a change in testosterone to cortisol ratio, an effect that was mostly driven by a reduction in cortisol concentrations. Thus, to date, no single compelling HPA-axis related biomarker has emerged from the literature focused specifically on athletic populations.
In summary, the results of research linking burnout with HPA-axis measures (e. g., cortisol) have been relatively unclear. However, some of the largest and more rigorous studies have hinted that biomarkers such as elevated morning (i. e., pre-work) cortisol levels, a blunted cortisol awakening response, and diminished salivary cortisol secretion in the afternoon and evening (e. g., at bedtime) might warrant further investigation [41]. It therefore remains for future research to assess these physiological markers alongside other indices (e. g., HRV).
#
Autonomic nervous system (ANS) metrics
Many studies searching for a biomarker of burnout have used ANS metrics [21]. The ANS plays a pivotal role in maintaining balance in key regulatory functions (e. g., metabolism, temperature), and is an important system that enables the body to react flexibly and effectively in response to stressors [59]. The ANS has two main branches: the sympathetic nervous system (SNS) which aids the rapid mobilization of energy to actively combat a stressor (i. e., fight vs. flight response), and the parasympathetic nervous system (PNS) which facilitates energy conservation and restoration following a stressor [60]. These two systems are normally in dynamic balance, but chronic stress results in the SNS persistently dominating the PNS, thereby placing excessive demands on the body and inhibiting energy conservation and recovery [61]. This stress-related ANS imbalance (i. e., SNS hyperactivity and PNS hypoactivity) has most commonly been examined via HRV, defined as variations in the time intervals between successive heart beats [62]. Both ANS branches impact HRV via the sinoatrial node, with SNS and PNS activity accelerating and decelerating heart rate, respectively [63]. Due to differences in chemical signaling at the sinoatrial node, the PNS can modulate heart rate via the vagus nerve on a shorter timescale than the SNS (i. e., milliseconds versus seconds), meaning that high-frequency changes in HRV offer a relatively purer measure of PNS activity, while low-frequency changes reflect a mixture of SNS and PNS activity [64].
Research linking burnout with low-frequency HRV parameters has revealed equivocal results [52] [59]. Indeed, while some studies have reported null effects [e. g., 38], others have found positive [e. g., 65] or negative [e. g., 66] associations. For instance, Thielmann et al. [67] performed a 24-hour electrocardiogram (ECG) recording with 414 employees working with patient or child populations and found no differences between low, moderate, and high burnout groups in low-frequency HRV. While May et al. [68] used a similar 24-hour ECG recording protocol with 88 female undergraduate students, they found that higher burnout was associated with greater low-frequency HRV after controlling for depression symptomology. In contrast, Lennartsson et al. [69] found that low-frequency HRV, assessed over a 300-second recording session in a supine position, was lower in a clinical burnout group compared to non-clinical high and low burnout groups. This unclear pattern of results could be due to low-frequency HRV metrics reflecting a blend of SNS and PNS activity, which contrasts with high-frequency HRV metrics that more purely indicate PNS activity (or vagal tone) [70]. Indeed, given the less clear physiological origin of low-frequency HRV (e. g., baroreflex activity, mix of SNS vs. PNS activity), researchers are encouraged to focus more on metrics linked to PNS activity [70].
Studies relating burnout with HRV metrics reflective of PNS activity have also revealed mixed findings [21] [59]. Indeed, studies using indices from the frequency-domain (e. g., high-frequency HRV) have either reported null effects [e. g., 71], positive associations [e. g., 29], or negative associations [e. g., 72]. However, frequency-domain HRV measures are susceptible to the influence of movement and respiration [59] [73], which might have clouded the results. To combat this, some studies have assessed only time-domain HRV metrics reflective of PNS activity (e. g., root mean square of successive differences; RMSSD) [70]. For instance, well-powered and high-quality studies have emerged from the Dresden Burnout Study [74]. In the first, Kanthak et al. [75] measured HRV among 410 healthy adults during three conditions (i. e., an emotionally arousing situation, a recumbent recovery period, and seated rest) and found that higher burnout (and especially exhaustion) was associated with lower RMSSD in most conditions after controlling for covariates (e. g., age). Subsequently, among 167 healthy adults, Wekenborg et al. [76] found that lower RMSSD during seated rest predicted higher burnout (and particularly exhaustion) 12 months later after accounting for covariates (e. g., gender). Finally, in a longitudinal study of 378 healthy adults, Wekenborg et al. [77] found that lower RMSSD predicted higher exhaustion, but not vice versa (or global burnout), after controlling for covariates (e. g., body mass index). Thus, RMSSD might offer a promising biomarker of burnout, and particularly the dimension of emotional exhaustion [78].
Although RMSSD may potentially mark burnout, when looking at the burnout-HRV literature overall, the findings have been mixed, likely due to high methodological heterogeneity (but possibly because burnout is not reflected in ANS-related indices) [52]. First, the way in which burnout has been assessed has varied between studies [21]. Indeed, while the MBI has been used most [e. g., 79], other instruments such as the Copenhagen burnout inventory [80] have also been used [e. g., 66]. Second, HRV recording equipment has differed across studies [81], ranging from gold-standard ECG devices [e. g., 82] to chest- or wrist-worn sensors [e. g., 83] and mobile applications using photoplethysmography [e. g., 84]. Third, the time periods used to record HRV data has varied [59], with most studies using 24-hour recordings [e. g., 38] but some using shorter measurement periods during rest [e. g., 85] or work-related tasks [e. g., 86]. Fourth, a vast array of HRV metrics has been used across studies, often with little justification [52]. For example, May et al. [68] found a negative association between burnout and very low-frequency HRV, with limited consideration of the metrics’ physiological origin (e. g., thermoregulation) [70]. Fifth, most studies have used absolute HRV values that do not account for participants’ normative values and, as such, have not explored other promising metrics (e. g., HRV coefficient of variation) [59]. Finally, many studies have been cross-sectional with small samples, thereby limiting causal understanding [21]. Given these issues, future work examining ANS-related indices of burnout should strive for greater methodological homogeneity (e. g., in equipment use) and utilize stronger methods (e. g., larger sample sizes). Unfortunately, there is a dearth of research on burnout and HRV in sport, and what studies do exist, suffer from similar issues (see [Table 1]). For example, in a rare study, Dobson et al. [87] found no link between burnout and RMSSD among 13 female swimmers.
In summary, the results of studies searching for an ANS-related indicator of burnout (e. g., HRV) have been equivocal. However, the most well-powered and rigorous studies imply that indices of PNS hypoactivity such as lower RMSSD during seated rest might hold some promise [e. g., 75,76]. Future research therefore needs to further assess this biomarker while taking into consideration the key issues central to this complex field of study.
#
Considerations for future research
While this review will help the selection of biomarkers for further investigation (e. g., bedtime salivary cortisol; [41]), such decisions should be theoretically grounded. Given that burnout is theorized to develop when there is a persistent mismatch between situational demands and personal coping resources [78], the biopsychosocial model (BPSM) of challenge and threat might offer a neat theoretical framework to explore potential biomarkers [88]. Indeed, according to the BPSM, a threat appraisal, where an individual feels that situational demands exceed their coping resources, is associated with a distinct physiological profile [89]. Specifically, compared to a challenge appraisal (i. e., when coping resources are deemed to match or exceed situational demands), a threat appraisal is marked by lower vagally-mediated HRV (e. g., RMSSD) and higher cortisol at rest [90]. Moreover, in response to an acutely stressful event (e. g., sporting competition), a threat appraisal results in cardiovascular reactivity characterized by relatively lower cardiac output (i. e., amount of blood ejected by the heart per minute) and higher total peripheral resistance (i. e., net constriction in the blood vessels) [89]. Thus, the BPSM could help researchers select and integrate potential biomarkers of burnout in future investigations, resulting in studies that are more theoretically underpinned. However, it should be noted that other frameworks exist [12], some of which go beyond stress-based explanations and offer additional causes of athlete burnout (e. g., unidimensional identity).
In addition to the predictions of the BPSM [91], other pertinent frameworks could guide future research on burnout biomarkers. Indeed, borrowing from models of allostatic load [92], a common hypothesis is that the early stages of burnout are marked by higher HPA-axis and ANS activity (e. g., hypercortisolism and SNS hyperactivity) [21], while over time these systems become exhausted due to prolonged or recurrent stress, resulting in more severe burnout being marked by lower HPA-axis and ANS activity (e. g., hypocortisolism and lower PNS activity) [36]. Thus, future research should assess if different biomarkers are needed to detect athletes at varying stages of burnout. To help with this feat, researchers could draw on initial conceptual models that suggested that burnout dimensions develop sequentially (i. e., emotional exhaustion → cynicism → professional inefficacy) [93]. As well as testing these theory-driven predictions, future work could explore if burnout biomarkers are more likely to emerge in response to challenges (e. g., intense exercise) [22]. Indeed, research has hinted that burnout may be marked by hypocortisolism in response to acute stress [e. g., 94], a response that differs from the typical increase in cortisol seen in reaction to a stressor [25].
As noted previously, the literature exploring burnout biomarkers has been hampered by cross-sectional designs and small samples [21]. Thus, future research should use longitudinal designs with larger samples [22]. Historically, this has been difficult given the expensive, time-consuming, laboratory-based, and expertise-reliant nature of biomarker testing (e. g., cortisol) [95]. However, new technology is emerging (e. g., microsensors) that might help researchers determine if biomarkers (e. g., cortisol, HRV) are useful in identifying burnt out athletes [96]. For example, due to learnings from the COVID-19 pandemic, companies are developing rapid assay-based tests that could, in the future, enable accurate, reliable, and cost-effective assessments of cortisol in day-to-day life [97]. While many of these diagnostic tests are only at the proof-of-concept stage, technology already exists that allows for the valid and expedient measurement of vagally-mediated HRV (e. g., camera-based photoplethysmography via applications such as HRV4Training) [98]. This technology might offer an effective way of integrating burnout biomarker testing into practice given that athletes are used to wearing devices (e. g., heart rate monitors) to supply sport scientists with data (e. g., related to physical training load). Although sport may provide an excellent “natural laboratory” to assess burnout biomarkers (e. g., cortisol, HRV), more research is needed into the acceptability of such testing and how it might integrate with existing practices (e. g., well-being monitoring) [99]. There is also a risk that, due to the relatively high cost of this technology (vs. self-report measures), such biomarker testing is only available to support elite professional athletes operating at the highest levels in their sport, with lower-level and talented youth athletes missing out.
As mentioned previously, high methodological heterogeneity has hindered burnout biomarker research [36]. Future work therefore needs to be more consistent in the methods used to assess burnout (e. g., conceptualization, measurement) and physiological markers (e. g., equipment, sample timing). Regarding the former and in line with past recommendations [100], future studies that examine burnout as a health problem in athletes should use a definition where exhaustion is central [e. g., 50] and use questionnaires that align with this conceptualization (e. g., SMBQ) [13]. That said, given recent research showing that athlete burnout is on the rise predominately due to increases in sport devaluation and a reduced sense of accomplishment (vs. emotional and physical exhaustion) [9], future investigations might also wish to use other additional questionnaires (e. g., ABQ) [14]. Furthermore, to encourage more complex approaches (e. g., experience sampling methods), researchers could develop expedient single-item measures (e. g., “Right now I feel exhausted”) [49]. Also, rather than dividing athletes into groups (e. g., high vs. low) using arbitrary cut-points or median splits, future work should examine associations between burnout and biomarkers continuously and report findings for global burnout as well as its subdimensions [7]. Indeed, this is important given that some biomarkers (e. g., RMSSD) have been more strongly linked to certain dimensions (e. g., exhaustion) [77]. While uniformity in physiological recording is biomarker-dependent, future research should use valid and reliable equipment (e. g., ECG devices for vagally mediated HRV), comparable measurement times and methods (e. g., cortisol samples taken via passive drool upon awakening, 30 minutes after waking, 2 PM, 4 PM, and bedtime), and similar metrics in analyses (e. g., RMSSD). As well as striving for greater homogeneity in the methods utilized, future work should strictly adhere to best-practice guidelines [70] [101].
Future burnout biomarker research can also be improved by more routinely controlling for confounding factors [21], either by accounting for covariates in protocols (e. g., asking participants not to consume caffeine two hours before measuring vagally mediated HRV) [70], or controlling for them statistically (e. g., adjusting for awakening time when assessing cortisol) [101]. Confounders of particular interest are likely to include age, gender, physical fitness, sleep time, and stage of season [36]. For age, research has shown that HRV declines with age, particularly for metrics reflecting PNS activity (e. g., RMSSD) [102]. Moreover, for gender, studies have suggested that chronically stressed females may display larger increases in cortisol upon awakening than males [e. g., 103]. Furthermore, for physical fitness, research has shown that highly fit athletes have higher resting HRV than less fit athletes [e. g., 104]. Additionally, for sleep, research has suggested that shorter sleep times may be associated with lower cortisol at awakening [e. g., 105]. Finally, for stage of season, research has shown that RMSSD might be higher among athletes during periods of overload training than at other times (e. g., tapering) [87]. Thus, to confidently link certain biomarkers to athlete burnout, future research needs to better account and control for important confounding factors [21]. Indeed, one crucial factor, which is central to most sport contexts, is the exercise associated with training and competition. Given that many biomarkers are impacted acutely by exercise (e. g., cortisol) [106], it is possible that physiological indices measured at rest and away from training or competition temporally (e. g., at bedtime) might hold most promise in identifying athlete burnout.
In summary, the key considerations for future research on physiological markers of athlete burnout include: (1) ensuring the selection of biomarkers is underpinned by empirical evidence, pertinent theory, and takes into account stage of burnout development; (2) using technology that enables biomarker data to be collected longitudinally from large samples; (3) adopting more homogenous methods in the assessment of burnout and physiological parameters; and (4) routinely controlling for confounding factors (see [Table 2]). The final point is particularly important given the myriad of factors that can impact biomarkers such as cortisol and HRV (e. g., diet, illness, medication) [35] [70]. If the factors listed above are adequately considered, a clearer picture should emerge of any physiological indices of athlete burnout.
Ensure selected biomarkers are: |
|
|
|
Use technology that enables: |
|
|
|
Adopt consistent methods when: |
|
|
|
Control for confounding factors, such as: |
|
|
|
|
|
|
#
#
Conclusion
Athlete burnout is a growing issue in high-performance sport and, given its negative consequences (e. g., mental ill-health), it is important to effectively monitor it to aid intervention efforts. While questionnaires are available (e. g., ABQ), the limitations associated with these subjective measures (e. g., social desirability bias) means that objective physiological markers may also be useful. Thus, this article summarized and critiqued existing literature on potential burnout biomarkers. Drawing largely on work outside of sport, it revealed a mixed picture, with few clear physiological indices, potentially due to the high methodological heterogeneity and weaknesses inherent in the literature (e. g., varying burnout measures, small sample sizes) but possibly because burnout does not leave a physiological footprint (or trace). While biomarkers are unlikely to offer a panacea for identifying burnt out athletes, some indices may be worthy of further exploration, including elevated morning cortisol levels, blunted cortisol awakening responses, diminished cortisol secretion in the afternoon and evening (e. g., at bedtime), and lower vagally mediated HRV (i. e., RMSSD) during seated rest. Important recommendations were also made for future research, including the need to adopt more homogenous methods in assessing burnout and physiological parameters (e. g., cortisol), and the use of technology that enables biomarker data to be collected longitudinally from larger samples (e. g., rapid assay-based tests). As a result, it is hoped that this article will spark a step change in research on the psychophysiology of athlete burnout and guide applied practice.
#
#
Conflict of Interest
Dr Isoard-Gautheur and Professor Gustafsson have no conflicts of interest to declare relevant to the content of this article. Although Dr Moore wrote this article while on secondment with an organisation interested in developing a biomarker test of burnout, this organisation did not have any input regarding the content of the article, which was drafted completely independently by the authors listed.
Acknowledgement
The research informing this article was funded by the University of Bath and developed based on the International Journal of Sports Medicine ethical standards (see Harriss et al., 2022).
-
References
- 1 Freudenberger HJ. Staff burn-out. J Soc Issues 1974; 30: 159-165
- 2 Maslach C. ed. Burnout: The cost of caring. New York: Prentice-Hall; 1982
- 3 Freudenberger HJ. The staff burn-out syndrome in alternative institutions. Psychother-Theor Res 1975; 12: 73-82
- 4 Feigley DA. Psychological burnout in high-level athletes. Phys Sportsmed 1984; 12: 108-119
- 5 Dale J, Weinberg R. Burnout in sport: A review and critique. J Appl Sport Psychol 1990; 2: 67-83
- 6 Raedeke TD. Is athlete burnout more than just stress? A sport commitment perspective. J Sport Exercise Psy 1997; 19: 396-417
- 7 Gerber M, Gustafsson H, Seelig H. et al. Usefulness of the Athlete Burnout Questionnaire (ABQ) as a screening tool for the detection of clinically relevant burnout symptoms among young elite athletes. Psychol Sport Exerc 2018; 39: 104-113
- 8 Gustafsson H, Kenttä G, Hassmén P. et al. Prevalence of burnout in competitive adolescent athletes. TSP 2007; 21: 21-37
- 9 Madigan DJ, Olsson LF, Hill AP. et al. Athlete burnout symptoms are increasing: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of average levels from 1997 to 2019. J Sport Exercise Psy 2022; 44: 153-168
- 10 Isoard-Gautheur S, Trouilloud D, Gustafsson H. et al. Associations between the perceived quality of the coach-athlete relationship and athlete burnout: An examination of the mediating role of achievement goals. Psychol Sport Exerc 2016; 22: 210-217
- 11 Glandorf HL, Madigan DJ, Kavanagh O. et al. Mental and physical health outcomes of burnout in athletes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int Rev Sport Exer P 2023;
- 12 Gustafsson H, DeFreese JD, Madigan DJ. Athlete burnout: Review and recommendations. Curr Opin Psychol 2017; 16: 109-113
- 13 Shirom A, Melamed S. A comparison of the construct validity of two burnout measures in two groups of professionals. Int J Stress Manag 2006; 13: 176-200 psycnet.apa.org/
- 14 Raedeke TD, Smith AL. Development and preliminary validation of an athlete burnout measure. J Sport Exercise Psy 2001; 23: 281-306
- 15 Madigan DJ, Gustafsson H, Smith A. et al. The BASES expert statement on burnout in sport. Sport Exerc Scient 2019; 61: 6-7 www.bases.org.uk/imgs/tses___issue_61_autumn_19_expert_statement_online__pages_579.pdf
- 16 Lonsdale C, Hodge K, Jackson SA. Athlete engagement: II. Developmental and initial validation of the Athlete Engagement Questionnaire. Int J Sport Psychol 2007; 38: 471-492
- 17 Isoard-Gautheur S, Martinent G, Guillet-Descas E. et al. Development and evaluation of the psychometric properties of a new measure of athlete burnout: The Athlete Burnout Scale. Int J Stress Manage 2018; 25: 108-123 psycnet.apa.org/
- 18 Mellano KT, Horn TS, Mann M. Examining links between coaching behaviours and collegiate athletes’ burnout levels using a longitudinal approach. Psychol Sport Exerc 2022; 61: 102189
- 19 Gustafsson H, Kenttä G, Hassmén P. et al. The process of burnout: A multiple case study of three elite endurance athletes. Int J Sport Psychol 2007; 38: 388-416
- 20 Baumeister RF. Self-esteem, self-presentation, and future interaction: A dilemma of reputation. J Pers 1982; 50: 29-45
- 21 Dahlman AS, Jonsdottir IH, Hansson C. The hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis and the autonomic nervous system in burnout. Handb Clin Neurol 2021; 182: 83-94
- 22 Jonsdottir IH, Dahlman AS. Endocrine and immunological aspects of burnout: A narrative review. Eur J Endocrinol 2019; 180: R147-R158
- 23 De Kloet ER, Joels M, Holsboer F. Stress and the brain: From adaptation to disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005; 6: 463-475
- 24 Bayes A, Tavella G, Parker G. The biology of burnout: Causes and consequences. The World J Biol Psychiatry 2021; 22: 686-698
- 25 Allen AP, Kennedy PJ, Cryan JF. et al. Biological and physiological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress Test. Neurosci Biobehav R 2014; 38: 94-124
- 26 Pruessner JC, Wolf OT, Hellhammer DH. et al. Free cortisol levels after awakening: A reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Sci 1997; 61: 2539-2549
- 27 McEwan BS. What is the confusion with cortisol?. Chronic Stress 2019; 3: 2470547019833647
- 28 Souza RO, Alves DL, Manoel FDA. et al. Analysis of dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate, cortisol, and testosterone levels in performance athletes affected by burnout syndrome. J Exerc Physiol Online 2018; 21: 150-156
- 29 Morgan CA, Cho T, Hazlett G. et al. The impact of burnout on human physiology and on operational performance: A prospective study of soldiers enrolled in the combat diver qualification course. Yale J Biol Med 2002; 75: 199-205
- 30 Moya-Albiol L, Serrano MA, Salvador A. Burnout as an important factor in the psychophysiological responses to a workday in teachers. Stress Health 2010; 26: 382-393
- 31 Begin AS, Hata S, Berkowitz LR. et al. Biomarkers of clinician burnout. J Gen Intern Med 2022; 37: 478-479
- 32 Melamed S, Ugarten U, Shirom A. et al. Chronic burnout, somatic arousal, and elevated salivary cortisol levels. J Psychosom Res 1999; 46: 591-598
- 33 Deneva T, Ianakiev Y, Keskinova D. Burnout syndrome in physicians: Psychological assessment and biomarker research. Medicina (Kaunas) 2019; 55: 209
- 34 Wingenfeld K, Schulz M, Damkroeger A. et al. Elevated diurnal salivary cortisol in nurses is associated with burnout but not with vital exhaustion. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34: 1144-1151
- 35 Stalder T, Kirschbaum C, Kudielka BM. et al. Assessment of the cortisol awakening responses: Expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 63: 414-432
- 36 Rothe N, Steffen J, Penz M. et al. Examination of peripheral basal and reactive cortisol levels in major depressive disorder and the burnout syndrome: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav R 2020; 114: 232-270
- 37 McCanlies EC, Leppma M, Mnatsakanova A. et al. Associations of burnout with awakening and diurnal cortisol among police officers. Compr Psychoneuroendocrinol 2020; 4: 100016
- 38 Traunmuller C, Stefitz R, Gaisbachgrabner K. et al. Psychophysiological concomitants of burnout: Evidence for different subtypes. J Psychosom Res 2019; 118: 41-48
- 39 Sjors A, Jonsdottir IH. No alterations in diurnal cortisol profiles before and during the treatment in patients with stress-related exhaustion. Int J Occup Med Env 2015; 28: 120-129
- 40 Marchand A, Durand P, Juster RP. et al. Workers’ psychological distress, depression, and burnout symptoms: Associations with diurnal cortisol profiles. Scand J Work Environ Health 2014; 40: 305-314 www.jstor.org/stable/43188021
- 41 Marchand A, Juster RP, Durand P. et al. Burnout symptom sub-types and cortisol profiles: What’s burning most?. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014; 40: 27-36
- 42 Parent-Lamarche A, Marchand A. Work stress, personality traits, and cortisol secretion: Testing a model for job burnout. Work 2018; 60: 485-497
- 43 Chida Y, Steptoe A. Cortisol awakening responses and psychosocial factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychol 2009; 80: 265-278
- 44 Anderson T, Wideman L, Cadegiani FA. et al. Effects of overtraining status on the cortisol awakening response – Endocrine and metabolic responses on overtraining syndrome (EROS-CAR). Int J Sport Physiol 2021; 16: 965-973
- 45 Bellingrath S, Weigl T, Kudielka BM. Cortisol dysregulation in schoolteachers in relation to burnout, vital exhaustion, and effort-reward-imbalance. Biol Psychol 2008; 78: 104-113
- 46 Colonnello V, Carnevali L, Russo PM. et al. Reduced recognition of facial emotional expressions in global burnout and burnout depersonalization in healthcare providers. PeerJ 2021; 9: e10610
- 47 Osterberg K, Karlson B, Hansen AM. Cognitive performance in patients with burnout, in relation to diurnal salivary cortisol. Stress 2009; 12: 70-81
- 48 Maslach C, Schaufeli WB, Leiter MP. Job burnout. Annu Rev Psychol 2001; 52: 397-422
- 49 Sonnenschein M, Mommersteeg PMC, Houtveen JH. et al. Exhaustion and endocrine functioning in clinical burnout: An in-depth study using the experience sampling method. Biol Psychol 2007; 75: 176-184
- 50 Shirom A. Job-related burnout: A review. In: Quick JC, Tetrick LE, eds. Handbook of Occupational Health Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2003. psycnet.apa.org/
- 51 Soderstrom M, Ekstedt M, Akerstedt T. Weekday and weekend patterns of diurnal cortisol, activation and fatigue among people scoring high for burnout. Scand J Work Environ Health 2006; 2: 35-40 www.sjweh.fi/article/1042
- 52 Danhof-Pont MB, Van Veen T, Zitman FG. Biomarkers in burnout: A systematic review. J Psychosom Res 2011; 70: 505-524
- 53 Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Burnout, perceived stress, and cortisol responses to awakening. Psychosom Med 1999; 61: 197-204
- 54 Sjors A, Ljung T, Jonsdottir IH. Long-term follow-up of cortisol awakening response in patients treated for stress-related exhaustion. BMJ Open 2012; 2: e001091
- 55 Davis L, Appleby R, Davis P. et al. The role of coach-athlete relationship quality in team sport athletes’ psychophysiological exhaustion: Implications for physical and cognitive performance. J Sport Sci 2018; 36: 1985-1992
- 56 Landolt K, Maruff P, Horan B. et al. Reduced professional efficacy is associated with a blunted salivary alpha-amylase awakening response. Physiol Behav 2019; 199: 292-299
- 57 Monfared SS, Lebeau JC, Mason J. et al. A bio-physio-psychological investigation of athletes’ burnout. Res Q Exercise Sport 2021; 92: 189-198
- 58 Martin J, Byrd B, Hew-Butler T. et al. A longitudinal study on the psychological and physiological predictors of burnout in NCAA collegiate swimmers. J App Sport Psychol 2022; 34: 1295-1311
- 59 De Looff PC, Cornet LJM, Embregts PJCM. et al. Associations of sympathetic and parasympathetic activity in job stress and burnout: A systematic review. PloS One 2018; 13: e0205741
- 60 Thayer JF, Hansen AL, Johnson BH. The non-invasive assessment of autonomic influences on the heart using impedance cardiography and heart rate variability. In: Steptoe A, ed. Handbook of Behavioural Medicine. New York: Springer; 2010
- 61 Thayer JF, Lane RD. Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci Biobehav R 2009; 33: 81-88
- 62 Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front Public Health 2017; 5: 258
- 63 Saul JP. Beat-to-beat variations of heart rate reflect modulation of cardiac autonomic outflow. Physiology 1990; 5: 32-37
- 64 Berntson GG, Bigger JT, Eckberg DL. et al. Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology 1997; 34: 623-648
- 65 Kalynychenko IO, Latina HO, Upsenka VM. et al. Physiological and hygienic correlates of teachers’ occupational burnout syndrome development. Wiad Lek 2021; 74: 1661-1665
- 66 Zhang M, Liu L, Shi Y. et al. Longitudinal associations of burnout with heart rate variability in patients following acute coronary syndrome: A one-year follow-up study. Gen Hosp Psychiat 2018; 53: 59-64
- 67 Thielmann B, Karlsen HR, Darius S. et al. Heart rate variability in different levels of burnout: Cross-sectional study of different occupational groups, heart rate variability, and burnout. J Occup Environ Med 2021; 63: e622-e630
- 68 May RW, Seibert GS, Sanchez-Gonzalez MA. et al. School burnout and heart rate variability: Risk of cardiovascular disease and hypertension in young adult females. Stress 2018; 21: 211-216
- 69 Lennartsson AK, Jonsdottir I, Sjors A. Low heart rate variability in patients with clinical burnout. Int J Psychophysiol 2016; 110: 171-178
- 70 Laborde S, Mosley E, Thayer JF. Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research: Recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Front Psychol 2017; 8: 213
- 71 Traunmuller C, Gaisbachgrabner K, Lackner HK. et al. Burnout of the mind – burnout of the body?. J Psychophysiol 2016; 32: 30-42
- 72 Shi Y, Jiang R, Zhu C. et al. High job burnout predicts low heart rate variability in the working population after a first episode of acute coronary syndrome. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2021; 18: 3431 www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/18/7/3431#
- 73 Hill LK, Sienbenbrock A, Sollers JJ. et al. Are all measures created equal? Heart rate variability and respiration. Biomed Sci Instrum 2009; 45: 71-76
- 74 Penz M, Wekenborg MK, Pieper L. et al. The Dresden burnout study: Protocol of a prospective cohort study for the bio-psychological investigation of burnout. Int J Meth Psych Res 2018; 27: e1613
- 75 Kanthak MK, Stalder T, Hill LB. et al. Autonomic dysregulation in burnout and depression: Evidence for the central role of exhaustion. Scand J Work Environ Health 2017; 43: 475-484 10.5271%2Fsjweh.3647
- 76 Wekenborg MK, Hill LK, Thayer JF. et al. The longitudinal association of reduced vagal tone with burnout. Psychosom Med 2019; 81: 791-798
- 77 Wekenborg MK, Schwerdtfeger A, Rothe N. et al. Determining the direction of prediction of the association between parasympathetic dysregulation and exhaustion symptoms. Sci Rep 2022; 12: 10648
- 78 Schaufeli WB, Leiter MP, Maslach C. Burnout: 35 years of research and practice. Career Dev Int 2009; 14: 204-220
- 79 De Vente W, Van Amsterdam JGC, Olff M. et al. Burnout is associated with reduced parasympathetic activity and reduced HPA axis responsiveness, predominately in males. BioMed Res Int 2015; 431725
- 80 Kristensen TS, Borritz M, Villadsen E. et al. The Copenhagen burnout inventory: A new tool for the assessment of burnout. Work Stress 2005; 19: 192-207
- 81 Orosz A, Federspiel A, Haisch S. et al. A biological perspective on differences and similarities between burnout and depression. Neurosci Biobehav R 2017; 73: 112-122
- 82 Schmid RF, Thomas J. Teachers’ ambulatory heart rate variability as an outcome and moderating variable in the job demands-resources model. Anxiety Stress Coping 2020; 33: 387-402
- 83 Barron LK, Moon MR. Impact of autonomic regulation on burnout and performance in thoracic surgery residents. JTCVS Open 2022; 11: 229-240
- 84 Afulani PA, Ongeri L, Kinyua J. et al. Psychological and physiological stress and burnout among maternity providers in a rural county in Kenya: Individual and situational predictors. BMC Public Health 2021; 21: 453
- 85 Gupta MD, Jha MK, Bansal A. et al. COVID-19-related burnout among healthcare workers in India and ECG based predictive machine learning model: Insights from the BRUCEE-Li study. Indian Heart J 2021; 73: 674-681
- 86 Nutor JJ, Aborigo RA, Okiring J. et al. Individual and situational predictors of psychological and physiological stress and burnout among maternity providers in Northern Ghana. PLoS One 2022; 17: e0278457
- 87 Dobson J, Harris B, Claytor A. et al. Selected cardiovascular and psychological changes throughout a competitive season in collegiate female swimmers. J Strength Cond Res 2020; 34: 3062-3069
- 88 Blascovich J, Tomaka J. The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. In: Zanna M, ed. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1996
- 89 Seery M. Challenge or threat? Cardiovascular indexes of resilience and vulnerability to potential stress in humans. Neurosci Biobehav R 2011; 35: 1603-1610
- 90 Mendes WB, Park J. Neurobiological concomitants of motivational states. Advances in Motivation Science 2014; 1: 233-270
- 91 Blascovich J. Challenge and threat. In: Elliot AJ, ed. Handbook of Approach and Avoidance Motivation. New York: Psychology Press; 2008
- 92 McEwan BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Ann NY Acad Sci 1998; 840: 33-44
- 93 Maslach C, Leiter MP. Understanding the burnout experience: Recent research and its implications for psychiatry. World Psychiatry 2016; 15: 103-111
- 94 Jonsson P, Osterberg K, Wallergard M. et al. Exhaustion-related changes in cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity to acute psychosocial stress. Physiol Behav 2015; 151: 327-337
- 95 Stalder T, Lupien SJ, Kudielka BM. et al. Evaluation and update of the expert consensus guidelines for the assessment of the cortisol awakening responses (CAR). Psychoneuroendocrinology 2022; 146: 105946
- 96 Kaur S, Gupta N, Malhotra BD. Recent developments in wearable and non-wearable point-of-case biosensors for cortisol detection. Expert Rev Mol Diagn 2023; 23: 217-230
- 97 Hogenelst K, Soeter M, Kallen V. Ambulatory measurement of cortisol: Where do we stand, and which way to follow?. Sens Bio-Sensing Res 2019; 22: 100249
- 98 Altini M, Plews D. What is behind changes in resting heart rate and heart rate variability? A large-scale analysis of longitudinal measurements acquired in free-living. Sensors 2021; 21: 7932
- 99 Duignan C, Doherty C, Caulfield B. et al. Single-item self-report measures of team-sport athlete wellbeing and their relationship with training load: A systematic review. J Athl Training 2020; 55: 944-953
- 100 Gustafsson H, Madigan DJ, Lundkvist E. Burnout in athletes. In: Fuchs R, Gerber M, eds. Handbuch Stressregulation und Sport. Berlin: Springer; 2018: 489-504 link.springer.com/chapter/
- 101 Laufer S, Engel S, Lupien S. et al. The Cortisol Assessment List (CoAL): A tool to systematically document and evaluate cortisol assessment in blood, urine, and saliva. Compr Psychoneuroendocrinol 2021; 9: 100108
- 102 Umetani K, Singer DH, McCraty R. et al. Twenty-four-hour time domain heart rate variability and heart rate: Relations to age and gender over nine decades. J Am Coll Cardiol 1998; 31: 593-601
- 103 Schulz P, Kirschbaum C, Pruessner J. et al. Increased free cortisol secretion after awakening in chronically stressed individuals due to work overload. Stress Health 1998; 14: 91-97 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1700(199804)14:2%3C91::AID-SMI765%3E3.0.CO;2-S
- 104 De Meersman RE. Heart rate variability and aerobic fitness. Am Heart J 1993; 125: 726-731
- 105 Vargas I, Lopez-Duran N. Dissecting the impact of sleep and stress on the cortisol awakening response in young adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014; 40: 10-16
- 106 Hough J, Leal D, Scott G. et al. Reliability of salivary cortisol and testosterone to a high-intensity cycling protocol to highlight overtraining. J Sport Sci 2021; 39: 2080-2086
Correspondence
Publication History
Received: 05 January 2024
Accepted after revision: 02 October 2024
Accepted Manuscript online:
02 October 2024
Article published online:
20 November 2024
© 2024. Thieme. All rights reserved.
Georg Thieme Verlag KG
Rüdigerstraße 14, 70469 Stuttgart, Germany
-
References
- 1 Freudenberger HJ. Staff burn-out. J Soc Issues 1974; 30: 159-165
- 2 Maslach C. ed. Burnout: The cost of caring. New York: Prentice-Hall; 1982
- 3 Freudenberger HJ. The staff burn-out syndrome in alternative institutions. Psychother-Theor Res 1975; 12: 73-82
- 4 Feigley DA. Psychological burnout in high-level athletes. Phys Sportsmed 1984; 12: 108-119
- 5 Dale J, Weinberg R. Burnout in sport: A review and critique. J Appl Sport Psychol 1990; 2: 67-83
- 6 Raedeke TD. Is athlete burnout more than just stress? A sport commitment perspective. J Sport Exercise Psy 1997; 19: 396-417
- 7 Gerber M, Gustafsson H, Seelig H. et al. Usefulness of the Athlete Burnout Questionnaire (ABQ) as a screening tool for the detection of clinically relevant burnout symptoms among young elite athletes. Psychol Sport Exerc 2018; 39: 104-113
- 8 Gustafsson H, Kenttä G, Hassmén P. et al. Prevalence of burnout in competitive adolescent athletes. TSP 2007; 21: 21-37
- 9 Madigan DJ, Olsson LF, Hill AP. et al. Athlete burnout symptoms are increasing: A cross-temporal meta-analysis of average levels from 1997 to 2019. J Sport Exercise Psy 2022; 44: 153-168
- 10 Isoard-Gautheur S, Trouilloud D, Gustafsson H. et al. Associations between the perceived quality of the coach-athlete relationship and athlete burnout: An examination of the mediating role of achievement goals. Psychol Sport Exerc 2016; 22: 210-217
- 11 Glandorf HL, Madigan DJ, Kavanagh O. et al. Mental and physical health outcomes of burnout in athletes: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Int Rev Sport Exer P 2023;
- 12 Gustafsson H, DeFreese JD, Madigan DJ. Athlete burnout: Review and recommendations. Curr Opin Psychol 2017; 16: 109-113
- 13 Shirom A, Melamed S. A comparison of the construct validity of two burnout measures in two groups of professionals. Int J Stress Manag 2006; 13: 176-200 psycnet.apa.org/
- 14 Raedeke TD, Smith AL. Development and preliminary validation of an athlete burnout measure. J Sport Exercise Psy 2001; 23: 281-306
- 15 Madigan DJ, Gustafsson H, Smith A. et al. The BASES expert statement on burnout in sport. Sport Exerc Scient 2019; 61: 6-7 www.bases.org.uk/imgs/tses___issue_61_autumn_19_expert_statement_online__pages_579.pdf
- 16 Lonsdale C, Hodge K, Jackson SA. Athlete engagement: II. Developmental and initial validation of the Athlete Engagement Questionnaire. Int J Sport Psychol 2007; 38: 471-492
- 17 Isoard-Gautheur S, Martinent G, Guillet-Descas E. et al. Development and evaluation of the psychometric properties of a new measure of athlete burnout: The Athlete Burnout Scale. Int J Stress Manage 2018; 25: 108-123 psycnet.apa.org/
- 18 Mellano KT, Horn TS, Mann M. Examining links between coaching behaviours and collegiate athletes’ burnout levels using a longitudinal approach. Psychol Sport Exerc 2022; 61: 102189
- 19 Gustafsson H, Kenttä G, Hassmén P. et al. The process of burnout: A multiple case study of three elite endurance athletes. Int J Sport Psychol 2007; 38: 388-416
- 20 Baumeister RF. Self-esteem, self-presentation, and future interaction: A dilemma of reputation. J Pers 1982; 50: 29-45
- 21 Dahlman AS, Jonsdottir IH, Hansson C. The hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal axis and the autonomic nervous system in burnout. Handb Clin Neurol 2021; 182: 83-94
- 22 Jonsdottir IH, Dahlman AS. Endocrine and immunological aspects of burnout: A narrative review. Eur J Endocrinol 2019; 180: R147-R158
- 23 De Kloet ER, Joels M, Holsboer F. Stress and the brain: From adaptation to disease. Nat Rev Neurosci 2005; 6: 463-475
- 24 Bayes A, Tavella G, Parker G. The biology of burnout: Causes and consequences. The World J Biol Psychiatry 2021; 22: 686-698
- 25 Allen AP, Kennedy PJ, Cryan JF. et al. Biological and physiological markers of stress in humans: Focus on the Trier Social Stress Test. Neurosci Biobehav R 2014; 38: 94-124
- 26 Pruessner JC, Wolf OT, Hellhammer DH. et al. Free cortisol levels after awakening: A reliable biological marker for the assessment of adrenocortical activity. Life Sci 1997; 61: 2539-2549
- 27 McEwan BS. What is the confusion with cortisol?. Chronic Stress 2019; 3: 2470547019833647
- 28 Souza RO, Alves DL, Manoel FDA. et al. Analysis of dehydroepiandrosterone sulphate, cortisol, and testosterone levels in performance athletes affected by burnout syndrome. J Exerc Physiol Online 2018; 21: 150-156
- 29 Morgan CA, Cho T, Hazlett G. et al. The impact of burnout on human physiology and on operational performance: A prospective study of soldiers enrolled in the combat diver qualification course. Yale J Biol Med 2002; 75: 199-205
- 30 Moya-Albiol L, Serrano MA, Salvador A. Burnout as an important factor in the psychophysiological responses to a workday in teachers. Stress Health 2010; 26: 382-393
- 31 Begin AS, Hata S, Berkowitz LR. et al. Biomarkers of clinician burnout. J Gen Intern Med 2022; 37: 478-479
- 32 Melamed S, Ugarten U, Shirom A. et al. Chronic burnout, somatic arousal, and elevated salivary cortisol levels. J Psychosom Res 1999; 46: 591-598
- 33 Deneva T, Ianakiev Y, Keskinova D. Burnout syndrome in physicians: Psychological assessment and biomarker research. Medicina (Kaunas) 2019; 55: 209
- 34 Wingenfeld K, Schulz M, Damkroeger A. et al. Elevated diurnal salivary cortisol in nurses is associated with burnout but not with vital exhaustion. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2009; 34: 1144-1151
- 35 Stalder T, Kirschbaum C, Kudielka BM. et al. Assessment of the cortisol awakening responses: Expert consensus guidelines. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2016; 63: 414-432
- 36 Rothe N, Steffen J, Penz M. et al. Examination of peripheral basal and reactive cortisol levels in major depressive disorder and the burnout syndrome: A systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav R 2020; 114: 232-270
- 37 McCanlies EC, Leppma M, Mnatsakanova A. et al. Associations of burnout with awakening and diurnal cortisol among police officers. Compr Psychoneuroendocrinol 2020; 4: 100016
- 38 Traunmuller C, Stefitz R, Gaisbachgrabner K. et al. Psychophysiological concomitants of burnout: Evidence for different subtypes. J Psychosom Res 2019; 118: 41-48
- 39 Sjors A, Jonsdottir IH. No alterations in diurnal cortisol profiles before and during the treatment in patients with stress-related exhaustion. Int J Occup Med Env 2015; 28: 120-129
- 40 Marchand A, Durand P, Juster RP. et al. Workers’ psychological distress, depression, and burnout symptoms: Associations with diurnal cortisol profiles. Scand J Work Environ Health 2014; 40: 305-314 www.jstor.org/stable/43188021
- 41 Marchand A, Juster RP, Durand P. et al. Burnout symptom sub-types and cortisol profiles: What’s burning most?. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014; 40: 27-36
- 42 Parent-Lamarche A, Marchand A. Work stress, personality traits, and cortisol secretion: Testing a model for job burnout. Work 2018; 60: 485-497
- 43 Chida Y, Steptoe A. Cortisol awakening responses and psychosocial factors: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Biol Psychol 2009; 80: 265-278
- 44 Anderson T, Wideman L, Cadegiani FA. et al. Effects of overtraining status on the cortisol awakening response – Endocrine and metabolic responses on overtraining syndrome (EROS-CAR). Int J Sport Physiol 2021; 16: 965-973
- 45 Bellingrath S, Weigl T, Kudielka BM. Cortisol dysregulation in schoolteachers in relation to burnout, vital exhaustion, and effort-reward-imbalance. Biol Psychol 2008; 78: 104-113
- 46 Colonnello V, Carnevali L, Russo PM. et al. Reduced recognition of facial emotional expressions in global burnout and burnout depersonalization in healthcare providers. PeerJ 2021; 9: e10610
- 47 Osterberg K, Karlson B, Hansen AM. Cognitive performance in patients with burnout, in relation to diurnal salivary cortisol. Stress 2009; 12: 70-81
- 48 Maslach C, Schaufeli WB, Leiter MP. Job burnout. Annu Rev Psychol 2001; 52: 397-422
- 49 Sonnenschein M, Mommersteeg PMC, Houtveen JH. et al. Exhaustion and endocrine functioning in clinical burnout: An in-depth study using the experience sampling method. Biol Psychol 2007; 75: 176-184
- 50 Shirom A. Job-related burnout: A review. In: Quick JC, Tetrick LE, eds. Handbook of Occupational Health Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 2003. psycnet.apa.org/
- 51 Soderstrom M, Ekstedt M, Akerstedt T. Weekday and weekend patterns of diurnal cortisol, activation and fatigue among people scoring high for burnout. Scand J Work Environ Health 2006; 2: 35-40 www.sjweh.fi/article/1042
- 52 Danhof-Pont MB, Van Veen T, Zitman FG. Biomarkers in burnout: A systematic review. J Psychosom Res 2011; 70: 505-524
- 53 Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Burnout, perceived stress, and cortisol responses to awakening. Psychosom Med 1999; 61: 197-204
- 54 Sjors A, Ljung T, Jonsdottir IH. Long-term follow-up of cortisol awakening response in patients treated for stress-related exhaustion. BMJ Open 2012; 2: e001091
- 55 Davis L, Appleby R, Davis P. et al. The role of coach-athlete relationship quality in team sport athletes’ psychophysiological exhaustion: Implications for physical and cognitive performance. J Sport Sci 2018; 36: 1985-1992
- 56 Landolt K, Maruff P, Horan B. et al. Reduced professional efficacy is associated with a blunted salivary alpha-amylase awakening response. Physiol Behav 2019; 199: 292-299
- 57 Monfared SS, Lebeau JC, Mason J. et al. A bio-physio-psychological investigation of athletes’ burnout. Res Q Exercise Sport 2021; 92: 189-198
- 58 Martin J, Byrd B, Hew-Butler T. et al. A longitudinal study on the psychological and physiological predictors of burnout in NCAA collegiate swimmers. J App Sport Psychol 2022; 34: 1295-1311
- 59 De Looff PC, Cornet LJM, Embregts PJCM. et al. Associations of sympathetic and parasympathetic activity in job stress and burnout: A systematic review. PloS One 2018; 13: e0205741
- 60 Thayer JF, Hansen AL, Johnson BH. The non-invasive assessment of autonomic influences on the heart using impedance cardiography and heart rate variability. In: Steptoe A, ed. Handbook of Behavioural Medicine. New York: Springer; 2010
- 61 Thayer JF, Lane RD. Claude Bernard and the heart-brain connection: Further elaboration of a model of neurovisceral integration. Neurosci Biobehav R 2009; 33: 81-88
- 62 Shaffer F, Ginsberg JP. An overview of heart rate variability metrics and norms. Front Public Health 2017; 5: 258
- 63 Saul JP. Beat-to-beat variations of heart rate reflect modulation of cardiac autonomic outflow. Physiology 1990; 5: 32-37
- 64 Berntson GG, Bigger JT, Eckberg DL. et al. Heart rate variability: Origins, methods, and interpretive caveats. Psychophysiology 1997; 34: 623-648
- 65 Kalynychenko IO, Latina HO, Upsenka VM. et al. Physiological and hygienic correlates of teachers’ occupational burnout syndrome development. Wiad Lek 2021; 74: 1661-1665
- 66 Zhang M, Liu L, Shi Y. et al. Longitudinal associations of burnout with heart rate variability in patients following acute coronary syndrome: A one-year follow-up study. Gen Hosp Psychiat 2018; 53: 59-64
- 67 Thielmann B, Karlsen HR, Darius S. et al. Heart rate variability in different levels of burnout: Cross-sectional study of different occupational groups, heart rate variability, and burnout. J Occup Environ Med 2021; 63: e622-e630
- 68 May RW, Seibert GS, Sanchez-Gonzalez MA. et al. School burnout and heart rate variability: Risk of cardiovascular disease and hypertension in young adult females. Stress 2018; 21: 211-216
- 69 Lennartsson AK, Jonsdottir I, Sjors A. Low heart rate variability in patients with clinical burnout. Int J Psychophysiol 2016; 110: 171-178
- 70 Laborde S, Mosley E, Thayer JF. Heart rate variability and cardiac vagal tone in psychophysiological research: Recommendations for experiment planning, data analysis, and data reporting. Front Psychol 2017; 8: 213
- 71 Traunmuller C, Gaisbachgrabner K, Lackner HK. et al. Burnout of the mind – burnout of the body?. J Psychophysiol 2016; 32: 30-42
- 72 Shi Y, Jiang R, Zhu C. et al. High job burnout predicts low heart rate variability in the working population after a first episode of acute coronary syndrome. Int J Environ Res Public Health 2021; 18: 3431 www.mdpi.com/1660-4601/18/7/3431#
- 73 Hill LK, Sienbenbrock A, Sollers JJ. et al. Are all measures created equal? Heart rate variability and respiration. Biomed Sci Instrum 2009; 45: 71-76
- 74 Penz M, Wekenborg MK, Pieper L. et al. The Dresden burnout study: Protocol of a prospective cohort study for the bio-psychological investigation of burnout. Int J Meth Psych Res 2018; 27: e1613
- 75 Kanthak MK, Stalder T, Hill LB. et al. Autonomic dysregulation in burnout and depression: Evidence for the central role of exhaustion. Scand J Work Environ Health 2017; 43: 475-484 10.5271%2Fsjweh.3647
- 76 Wekenborg MK, Hill LK, Thayer JF. et al. The longitudinal association of reduced vagal tone with burnout. Psychosom Med 2019; 81: 791-798
- 77 Wekenborg MK, Schwerdtfeger A, Rothe N. et al. Determining the direction of prediction of the association between parasympathetic dysregulation and exhaustion symptoms. Sci Rep 2022; 12: 10648
- 78 Schaufeli WB, Leiter MP, Maslach C. Burnout: 35 years of research and practice. Career Dev Int 2009; 14: 204-220
- 79 De Vente W, Van Amsterdam JGC, Olff M. et al. Burnout is associated with reduced parasympathetic activity and reduced HPA axis responsiveness, predominately in males. BioMed Res Int 2015; 431725
- 80 Kristensen TS, Borritz M, Villadsen E. et al. The Copenhagen burnout inventory: A new tool for the assessment of burnout. Work Stress 2005; 19: 192-207
- 81 Orosz A, Federspiel A, Haisch S. et al. A biological perspective on differences and similarities between burnout and depression. Neurosci Biobehav R 2017; 73: 112-122
- 82 Schmid RF, Thomas J. Teachers’ ambulatory heart rate variability as an outcome and moderating variable in the job demands-resources model. Anxiety Stress Coping 2020; 33: 387-402
- 83 Barron LK, Moon MR. Impact of autonomic regulation on burnout and performance in thoracic surgery residents. JTCVS Open 2022; 11: 229-240
- 84 Afulani PA, Ongeri L, Kinyua J. et al. Psychological and physiological stress and burnout among maternity providers in a rural county in Kenya: Individual and situational predictors. BMC Public Health 2021; 21: 453
- 85 Gupta MD, Jha MK, Bansal A. et al. COVID-19-related burnout among healthcare workers in India and ECG based predictive machine learning model: Insights from the BRUCEE-Li study. Indian Heart J 2021; 73: 674-681
- 86 Nutor JJ, Aborigo RA, Okiring J. et al. Individual and situational predictors of psychological and physiological stress and burnout among maternity providers in Northern Ghana. PLoS One 2022; 17: e0278457
- 87 Dobson J, Harris B, Claytor A. et al. Selected cardiovascular and psychological changes throughout a competitive season in collegiate female swimmers. J Strength Cond Res 2020; 34: 3062-3069
- 88 Blascovich J, Tomaka J. The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. In: Zanna M, ed. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. New York: Academic Press; 1996
- 89 Seery M. Challenge or threat? Cardiovascular indexes of resilience and vulnerability to potential stress in humans. Neurosci Biobehav R 2011; 35: 1603-1610
- 90 Mendes WB, Park J. Neurobiological concomitants of motivational states. Advances in Motivation Science 2014; 1: 233-270
- 91 Blascovich J. Challenge and threat. In: Elliot AJ, ed. Handbook of Approach and Avoidance Motivation. New York: Psychology Press; 2008
- 92 McEwan BS. Stress, adaptation, and disease: Allostasis and allostatic load. Ann NY Acad Sci 1998; 840: 33-44
- 93 Maslach C, Leiter MP. Understanding the burnout experience: Recent research and its implications for psychiatry. World Psychiatry 2016; 15: 103-111
- 94 Jonsson P, Osterberg K, Wallergard M. et al. Exhaustion-related changes in cardiovascular and cortisol reactivity to acute psychosocial stress. Physiol Behav 2015; 151: 327-337
- 95 Stalder T, Lupien SJ, Kudielka BM. et al. Evaluation and update of the expert consensus guidelines for the assessment of the cortisol awakening responses (CAR). Psychoneuroendocrinology 2022; 146: 105946
- 96 Kaur S, Gupta N, Malhotra BD. Recent developments in wearable and non-wearable point-of-case biosensors for cortisol detection. Expert Rev Mol Diagn 2023; 23: 217-230
- 97 Hogenelst K, Soeter M, Kallen V. Ambulatory measurement of cortisol: Where do we stand, and which way to follow?. Sens Bio-Sensing Res 2019; 22: 100249
- 98 Altini M, Plews D. What is behind changes in resting heart rate and heart rate variability? A large-scale analysis of longitudinal measurements acquired in free-living. Sensors 2021; 21: 7932
- 99 Duignan C, Doherty C, Caulfield B. et al. Single-item self-report measures of team-sport athlete wellbeing and their relationship with training load: A systematic review. J Athl Training 2020; 55: 944-953
- 100 Gustafsson H, Madigan DJ, Lundkvist E. Burnout in athletes. In: Fuchs R, Gerber M, eds. Handbuch Stressregulation und Sport. Berlin: Springer; 2018: 489-504 link.springer.com/chapter/
- 101 Laufer S, Engel S, Lupien S. et al. The Cortisol Assessment List (CoAL): A tool to systematically document and evaluate cortisol assessment in blood, urine, and saliva. Compr Psychoneuroendocrinol 2021; 9: 100108
- 102 Umetani K, Singer DH, McCraty R. et al. Twenty-four-hour time domain heart rate variability and heart rate: Relations to age and gender over nine decades. J Am Coll Cardiol 1998; 31: 593-601
- 103 Schulz P, Kirschbaum C, Pruessner J. et al. Increased free cortisol secretion after awakening in chronically stressed individuals due to work overload. Stress Health 1998; 14: 91-97 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1700(199804)14:2%3C91::AID-SMI765%3E3.0.CO;2-S
- 104 De Meersman RE. Heart rate variability and aerobic fitness. Am Heart J 1993; 125: 726-731
- 105 Vargas I, Lopez-Duran N. Dissecting the impact of sleep and stress on the cortisol awakening response in young adults. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2014; 40: 10-16
- 106 Hough J, Leal D, Scott G. et al. Reliability of salivary cortisol and testosterone to a high-intensity cycling protocol to highlight overtraining. J Sport Sci 2021; 39: 2080-2086