CC BY-NC-ND 4.0 · Sleep Sci 2024; 17(04): e357-e369
DOI: 10.1055/s-0044-1782525
Original Article

The Effects of Experimental Sleep Extension in Middle-to-Older-Aged Healthy Sleepers

1   Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, United States
,
Zachary R. Seymour
2   Walter Reed National Military Medical Center, Bethesda, MD, United States
,
Imran H. Iftikhar
3   Division of Pulmonary, Allergy, Critical Care & Sleep Medicine, Emory University, Atlanta, GA, United States
4   Atlanta Veterans Affairs Medical Center, Atlanta, GA, United States
,
M. Michele Burnette
1   Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, United States
,
Jennifer M. C. Vendemia
1   Department of Psychology, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, United States
,
Shawn D. Youngstedt
5   Edson College of Nursing and Health Innovation, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, United States
› Author Affiliations
Funding Source This work was partially supported by: R01 HL095799
 

Abstract

Objectives To determine the feasibility for middle-aged and older adults to extend their time in bed by 2 h per night for 3 consecutive weeks. Other aims were to examine the effects of sleep extension on mood, cognitive performance, and cardiovascular health.

Methods Ten healthy middle-aged to older adults (9 women; M = 65.20 ± 4.78 years) who reported regularly sleeping 6 to 8 h per night participated in a randomized controlled cross-over study: 3 weeks of both habitual sleep and extended sleep (1-week recovery between treatments). Participants were asked to spend 2 additional hours in bed per night during sleep extension. Cognitive (e.g., errors, response time), psychological (e.g., depression, anxiety, mood), and physiological measures (e.g., inflammation, glucose, triglycerides, blood pressure) were assessed.

Results Compared with habitual sleep, time in bed increased 81.63 ± 33.11 min and total sleep time increased 66.33 ± 28.64 min during sleep extension; these variables did not significantly change during baseline or the habitual sleep treatment. No significant treatment differences were found in the cognitive, psychological, or physiological measures.

Discussion Neither significant positive nor negative effects of sleep extension were found for any of the variables. In terms of feasibility, it was difficult for the participants to extend their time in bed and, subsequently, attain more sleep by the targeted amount. Sleep extension by a greater degree or longer period of times might be more likely to elicit positive or negative effects.


#

Introduction

Epidemiological studies have consistently shown that both short (< 6h) and long sleep (> 8h) are associated with mortality[1] and morbidity, including cardiovascular disease,[2] diabetes,[3] depressed mood,[4] and impaired cognitive function,[5] whereas approximately 7 to 8 h of sleep has been associated with the lowest health risks.[3] [6] [7] [8] Additionally, prospective studies (e.g., Ferrie et al.[9]) have found that compared with baseline, decreases and increases in sleep duration have been associated with increased mortality risk.

Multiple studies have indicated that the risks associated with long sleep are at least as great, if not greater, than the risks of short sleep.[10] [11] Nonetheless, the notion that long sleep is an underlying cause of mortality and morbidity has been challenged. The “reverse causality” explanation (i.e., illness causing long sleep) has been advanced,[12] but many studies of long sleepers have extensively controlled for health and risk factors. Moreover, some prospective studies established good health of the participants at baseline.[13] [14] However, it is noteworthy that these epidemiological studies were observational; therefore, no causal inferences can be made between sleep duration and health risks from these studies.

The argument that there are no plausible mechanisms to explain how chronic long sleep could be hazardous[12] [15] has also been contradicted by multiple lines of evidence. Indeed, just 2 to 5 days of bedrest, which leads to increased sleep, elicits significant impairments in insulin sensitivity and cardiovascular function[16] [17]; chronic exposure to periods of greater than 1h per day of completely sedentary behavior is associated with health risks[18] and worse sleep.[19] Furthermore, long sleep is associated with relatively low levels of daytime physical activity, especially in older individuals.[20]

Long sleep is associated with increased sleep fragmentation, which is linked to poor health outcomes in epidemiological and experimental studies.[21] Long sleep has also been associated with inflammatory markers,[22] possibly attributable to sleep fragmentation. It has been suggested that the proinflammatory cytokines (e.g., interleukin-6, IL-6) may mediate feelings of lethargy[23] which could, in turn, lead to further cytokine imbalance.[23]

Reduction in light exposure as a result of extended sleep could also lead to depression,[24] delayed circadian time, and a shorter perceived day length. Reductions in photoperiods have been associated with impaired health in some species.[25] Furthermore, depression and poorer cognitive function have been associated with longer sleep duration in adults 65 years and older.[26] Poorer cognitive function has also been found in middle-aged adults compared to age-matched adults with average sleep duration,[5] [27] [28] [29] and a decline in cognitive function (advanced cognitive aging) over time has been found in long sleepers.[5] [28] [29] One potential explanation for the link between sleep duration, cognition, and aging is increased amyloid-β burden.[27] Sleep fragmentation is another potential explanation for the link between lower cognitive function and long sleep duration.[27]

Also contradicted by the literature is the argument that there is no experimental evidence that it is harmful for a healthy adult to sleep 9 or more hours.[15] Several experimental studies of normal duration sleepers have found negative effects of 1 to 2 nights of 10+ h of sleep duration on cognitive function,[30] [31] and 2h of extended sleep for one week has had negative effects on depressed mood[32] and IL-6 inflammatory markers.[32] Nonetheless, the literature is relatively sparse,[33] with multiple limitations, including limited focus on objective measures of health. However, recent studies of average sleepers conducted by Gonzales et al.[34] and Clark et al.[35] found no effects of 5 to 6 nights of sleep extension (from 8–10 hours) on multiple measures of cardiovascular health or cognitive function.

It is noteworthy that experimental manipulation of sleep duration in these studies was short-term and intended to impact rapidly reversible outcomes. The results of these studies may not be directly comparable to long-term increases in sleep duration.

Additionally, it has been hypothesized that long sleepers likely experience an increased sleep pressure that leads to the perceived need for more sleep. In experimental sleep extension studies of healthy individuals, “forcing” sleep (as opposed to increased pressure) may not necessarily be directly comparable to individuals with increased sleep pressure. On the other hand, some evidence indicates that longer sleep in older adults partly reflects merely an assumption that they need at least 8 hours of sleep.[36]

Thus, it remains debatable whether long sleep truly causes negative effects. There is a particular need to investigate this question in middle-aged and older adults (≥ 65 years old) for whom the largest association of long sleep with health risks has been found. On the other hand, it has been argued that despite documented declines in objective sleep with aging,[37] [38] there is an equal need for sleep with aging,[39] and that older adults would benefit from sleep extension.

One aim of this sleep extension study was to determine whether it was feasible for middle-aged and older adults to extend time in bed (TIB) and consequently increase total sleep time (TST). Other aims were to examine the effects of 3 weeks sleep extension (2 hours) on mood state, cognitive performance, and cardiovascular and metabolic health.


#

Materials and Methods

Trial Design

This randomized controlled trial utilized a cross-over design. The study was reviewed and approved by the University of South Carolina (USC) Institutional Review Board ethics committee (approval number: Pro00024377).


#

Participants

Fourteen individuals who met the inclusion criteria were assessed. Three participants dropped out of the study due to schedule changes and/or time constraints. Eleven participants completed the study and were assessed on cognitive, emotional, and inflammatory measures in a cross-over design. One participant was excluded from data analysis due to non-compliance to the study protocol (participant slept less than 5 hours per night on average), leaving 10 participants included in the final analyses (1 male; ages ranged from 54–70 years old, M = 65.20 ± 4.78 years). Self-reported TST was an average of 7.00 ± 0.745, and body mass index averaged 27.33 ± 3.66.

Screening and Exclusions

Participants were initially screened with a brief phone interview to indicate interest and basic age, health, and sleeping inclusion criteria. To reduce the chance of expectancy or demand biases, prospective participants were informed that the intervention could have positive effects, negative effects, or no effects. Prospective participants who met the initial criteria (n = 30) were invited to the laboratory for a more extensive study orientation and screening.

After signing an informed consent form approved by the University of South Carolina's Institutional Review Board (IRB), prospective participants completed screening questionnaires, tests of cognitive function, and they were interviewed about their sleep, health, and behavioral patterns (i.e., self-reported usual sleep patterns, mental health, general health, etc.). The final screen involved an interview with a sleep physician (n = 15), who assessed participants for serious health conditions and sleep disorders, such as sleep apnea. Despite the interview with a sleep physician, it is possible that participants may have had sleep conditions like sleep apnea since sleep tests or polysomnography was not administered, which is a limitation of the current study.

Inclusion criteria were ages between 50 and 79, no significant health conditions, normal cognitive function (Mini-Mental State Examination ≤ 26), no known sleep disorders or diseases, and typically sleeping between 6 and 8 hours per night. Sleep duration of 6 to 8 h falls within the healthy, typical sleep duration of middle-aged-to-older adults.[38] Exclusion criteria included recent shiftwork (previous 2 months) or transmeridian travel (previous 4 weeks), or plans of either during the study period; use of hypnotics or other medications to promote sleep; any medical, neurologic, or psychiatric illness causing long sleep; factors associated with significant changes in inflammation (a key outcome variable), including several medical disorders (e.g., rheumatoid arthritis), medications (e.g., steroids) current smoking, obesity (body mass index > 33), and greater than 3 hours of moderate-to-vigorous exercise per week. Demographic information was collected at the time of consent as well as a list of all medications taken.


#
#

Interventions

Baseline

During a 1-week baseline, before each treatment, participants were asked to follow their usual sleep-wake schedules. During baseline, pretreatment measures were assessed, and baseline also served as a final screen to establish that the participants slept 6 to 8 hours. Participants were excluded if actigraphic TIB was outside of the 6 to 8 hour range.


#

Experimental Treatments and Randomization

Following baseline, participants were randomly assigned to 1 of 2 3-week treatments: (1) extended TIB, or (2) habitual TIB: The randomization was stratified by median duration of actigraphic baseline TST (< 7 vs. ≥ 7 h). At the end of each week, a research staff member met with the participant to download actigraph data, review the sleep log, encourage adherence to the treatments, and troubleshoot any difficulties.


#

Randomization

The primary author conducted the randomizations, which were stratified by median duration of actigraphic baseline TST (< 7 vs. 7 h). The primary author also enrolled participants on the study and assigned them to interventions.


#

Blinding

The trained clinical psychology graduate students were blinded in order to remove bias during the clinical assessments. Blinding was achieved by only supplying the participant number (with no indication of treatment assignment or the condition completed). The primary author and other staff who assistedwith conducting weekly visitswere not blinded to treatment order.

Extended TIB Treatment

In the extended TIB treatment, participants were asked to follow a fixed sleep schedule and extend their TIB by 2 hours greater than their median baseline actigraphic TIB. This was accomplished by advancing bedtime, delaying arising time, or some combination of these changes in accordance with the preferences and sleep tendencies of each participant. Participants were encouraged to extend sleep in a way that was convenient for them (e.g., go to bed earlier, wake later, or a combination of the two), and consistent with their individual sleep patterns. For example, if they experienced sleepiness in the evening or trouble arising in the morning, it was suggested that they might try going to bed earlier and sleeping later, respectively. They were also asked to remain consistent throughout the sleep extension weeks. Four participants extended sleep through a combination of earlier bedtimes and later wake-times, three participants extended sleep by waking later, two participants had earlier bedtimes, and one participant's data for bedtime/wake time were unavailable. There were no patterns based on group order.


#

Habitual TIB Treatment

In the habitual sleep condition, participants were asked to follow a fixed sleep schedule, in which their TIB was the same as their median baseline TIB.


#

Cross-over Design

After completion of the baseline and the first 3-week treatment, participants completed a 1-week recovery period to avoid carry-over effects. Participants then underwent a second 1-week baseline and crossed over to the other treatment. Participants were compensated $200 (prorated) for completing the study.


#
#
#

Outcomes

[Table 1] displays the time points in which each assessment occurred.

Table 1

Experimental measures during 1-week baseline and 3-week sleep extension and control groups cross-over study. Recovery week occurred during week 5.

Baseline

Treatment (Control vs. Sleep Extension)

Follow-up appts.

Follow-up appts.

Condition follow-up 1

Daily week 1

End week 1

Daily weeks 2–4

End week 2-4

End week 4

Actigraphy

Cognitive battery

Actigraphy

Sleepiness

Blood draw

Sleep diary

Sleepiness

Sleep diary

Fatigue

Cognitive battery

Mood diary

Fatigue

Mood diary

Depression

Depression

Anxiety

Anxiety

BP HR

BP HR

Baseline

Treatment (Control vs. Sleep Extension)

Follow-up appts.

Follow-up appts.

Condition follow-up 2

Daily week 6

End week 6

Daily weeks 7-9

End week 7-9

End week 9

Actigraphy

Sleepiness

Actigraphy

Sleepiness

Blood draw

Sleep diary

Fatigue

Sleep diary

Fatigue

Cognitive battery

Mood diary

Depression

Mood diary

Depression

Anxiety

Anxiety

BP HR

BP HR

Notes: Sleepiness (weekly): Epworth Sleepiness Scale; Depression: Beck Depression Inventory II (BDI-II; weekly), Hamilton Depression Scale (end of each baseline and end of each treatment); Anxiety: State Trait Anxiety Scale (weekly), Hamilton Anxiety Scale (end of each baseline and end of each treatment); Cognitive Battery (end of first baseline and end of each treatment): Block Pattern Memory Task, Sentence Memory Task, Trail Making Test, Psychomotor Vigilance Test, Stroop Test; BP (blood pressure) and HR (heart rate) assessed weekly.


Sleep Variables

Actigraphic Sleep

Throughout each baseline and each 3-week treatment, participants wore actigraphic wrist monitors (Phillips Respironics Actiwatch Spectrum device – Philips Respironics, Murrysville, PA, USA) for continuous assessment of sleep-wake patterns. Total sleep time, TIB, sleep fragmentation index, sleep efficiency, and sleep onset latency were also assessed using the Cole-Kripke algorithm.[40] Actigraphic algorithms calculated sleep fragmentation index as the sum of the percentage of mobile bouts and percentage of immobile bouts less than 1 minute to the number of immobile bouts. Sleep efficiency was calculated by the actigraph as the percentage of time spent asleep within the TIB interval.

Napping behavior was evaluated by examining activity outside of the nighttime sleep times. Inactivity over 30 consecutive minutes was considered to be potential napping behavior. Although participants were encouraged to use an event marker button on the actiwatches, few participants consistently used the button outside of nighttime sleep/wake. Overall, participants likely napped minimally throughout the study, but three participants may have reduced napping behavior slightly during the sleep extension weeks. Participants were reminded to keep their same habits throughout the habitual sleep and sleep extension weeks, so if they napped during habitual weeks, they were to continue napping per their usual schedule. Actigraphy indicated that a few participants were inactive during the afternoons or evenings (which could be interpreted as napping behavior), but most napped in the morning or afternoon inconsistently at 1 to 2 times per week.


#

Daily Sleep Diary

In a daily morning sleep diary, participants reported TIB, wake time, time out of bed, TST, and overall quality of sleep.


#
#

Psychological Assessments

Clinical Assessment of Depressed Mood and Anxiety:

At the end of each baseline and the end of each treatment period, supervised, blinded, and trained clinical psychology graduate students rated the participants with the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale[41] and the Hamilton Anxiety Rating Scale.[42] The Hamilton Depression Scale contains 21 items (first 17 scored) and must be administered by a trained clinician; higher scores indicate more severe depression (score ranges include: > 7 no depression, 7 to 17 mild depression, 18 to 24 moderate depression, and 25+ severe depression). The Hamilton Anxiety Scale scores range from 0 to 56, and it must be administered by a trained clinician; higher scores indicate more severe anxiety (score ranges include: < 17 mild anxiety, 18 to 24 mild-moderate anxiety, and 25 to 30 moderate-severe anxiety).


#

Weekly Psychological Questionnaires

The participants completed the following questionnaires at the end of each baseline week and the end of each treatment week. The Beck Depression Inventory (BDI-II)[43] is a 21-item self-administered questionnaire that assesses depressive symptoms. Scores range from 0 to 63, with higher scores indicating more severe depressive symptoms (cut-off score ranges include 0–13 minimal, 14–19 mild, 20–28 moderate, and 29–63 severe). The Spielberger State Trait Anxiety Inventory (Form Y-1)[44] is a self-administered survey that contains 20 items and assesses symptoms of state-trait anxiety; higher scores indicate greater severity of state and trait anxiety symptoms (score ranges include 20–37 no or low anxiety, 38–44 moderate anxiety, and 45–80 high anxiety). The Epworth Sleepiness Scale[45] is a self-administered 8-item measure of daytime sleepiness, with higher scores representing increased level of daytime sleepiness (score ranges include 0–5 normal daytime sleepiness, 6–10 higher normal daytime sleepiness, 11–12 mild excessive daytime sleepiness, 13–15 moderate excessive daytime sleepiness, and 16–24 severe excessive daytime sleepiness). The Multidimensional Assessment of Fatigue Scale (MAF)[46] is a self-administered scale of fatigue; higher scores on this scale indicate increased level of fatigue.


#

Daily Psychological Assessment

The participants completed a daily diary consisting of Likert scale questions pertaining to mood. They rated how they felt the previous day; a score of zero represented normal mood, -3 represented low mood, and +3 represented elated mood.


#
#

Cognitive Assessments

At the initial baseline and at the end of each 3-week treatment, working memory, sustained attention, and executive function were assessed with a cognitive performance battery. Two working memory tasks were used from the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, 5th edition[47]: Block Pattern Memory Task, which tests nonverbal working memory, and the Sentence Memory Task, which tests verbal working memory. Composite scores were obtained by scoring both the verbal and nonverbal tasks, but individual scores were determined to compare verbal and nonverbal working memory ability. In order to maintain the integrity of the tests, the Stanford-Binet 5 working memory assessments were administered by two blinded, supervised clinical psychology doctoral students.

The Psychomotor Vigilance Test, which is sensitive to aging and changes in sleep,[48] was used to assess changes in reaction time associated with state dependent changes in sustained attention. The metrics that were assessed included median reaction time (RT), the fastest 10% of responses, the slowest 10% of responses, the number of response lapses (RT > 500 ms), and standard deviation of responses.

The Stroop Color-Word Test[49] was used to measure executive functioning and cognitive control and flexibility.[50] Interference was measured by subtracting the average time of the first two subtasks completion from the average time of third subtask completion (Interference = [(Stroop I + Stroop II) / 2] – Stroop III).[50] [51] Reaction times and errors were also evaluated.

The Trail Making Test (parts A and B) was used in the cognitive battery. Specifically, Trail Making Test part B has been shown to be highly sensitive to cognitive aging and is a valid measure of executive function.[52] The reaction times of Trail Making Test completion were evaluated.


#

Physiological Measures

At the end of each 3-week intervention, participants underwent a 12-hr fast and blood draw (7 ml) to obtain lipid levels (lipoprotein, low-density lipoprotein, very low-density lipoprotein, and triglycerides), glucose, and an inflammation marker C-reactive protein. These assessments were all done in the morning to limit diurnal variation in these measures.

Following a 10-minute seated resting period, systolic and diastolic blood pressure, as well as heart rate, were recorded at the beginning of each weekly appointment with an Omron RS8 automatic wrist blood pressure monitor. The Omron Corporation, Kyoto Head Office, Shiokoji Horikawa, Shimogyo ku, Kyoto 600-8530, JAPAN RS8 device is a validated, easy to use blood pressure monitoring device.[53] At the beginning of each participant's weekly appointment, the device was placed on the participant's non-dominant wrist below the ulna wrist bone, per the manufacturer's instructions. After 10 minutes of quietly sitting, the blood pressure and heart rate readings were recorded, and then repeated after 2 minutes. If an error or large difference occurred between the two readings (i.e., > 10 units), the reading was taken a third time; averages of the recordings were reported.

Marker of Inflammation:

Recent infection/illness resulted in interpolation of data based on group values (n = 2; see Data Screening). Participants were carefully screened for alcohol consumption, smoking history, body mass index, physical activity, and use of medications (antidepressants, statins, and non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drug; NSAID),[54] as these factors may impact inflammatory marker results.


#
#
#

Randomization

The primary author conducted the randomizations, which were stratified by median duration of actigraphic baseline TST (< 7 vs. ≥ 7 h). The primary author also enrolled participants on the study and assigned them to interventions.


#

Blinding

The trained clinical psychology graduate students were blinded in order to remove bias during the clinical assessments. Blinding was achieved by only supplying the participant number (with no indication of treatment assignment or the condition completed). The primary author and other staff who assisted with conducting weekly visits were not blinded to treatment order.


#

Statistical Methods (Data Screening and Cleaning)

Data were initially screened for outliers (±3 standard deviations from the mean) and/or missing pieces of information. The reported data are from the 10 participants who completed the entire study.

Prior to analysis, sleep, cognitive, mood, and physiological measures were screened for equipment errors, accuracy of data entry, missing values, outliers, and fit between their distributions and the assumptions of analysis (including skewness and kurtosis) using the SPSS. Actigraph malfunction resulted in the loss of one week of an individual's sleep data from the last habitual sleep week treatment. The sleep data for the week were replaced by the mean from the same participant's two other habitual sleep weeks. The actigraph algorithm resulted in consistently implausibly short sleep onset durations data; therefore, sleep onset data were removed from analyses. Actigraphic data were cleaned by one researcher, who used the sleep diaries to corroborate the actigraphic data. Missing values of two participants for the Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, 5th edition, at baseline and for the end of the habitual sleep condition were replaced by mean scores of participants during the same condition and in the same condition order.

Missing values of one participant for the Hamilton Anxiety and Depression Scales for the end of the habitual sleep condition were replaced by mean scores of participants during the same condition and in the same condition order. The single missing value of one response on the BDI-II was replaced by the mean response to the other BDI-II questions for that week.

Two participants reported infections during the blood draw at the end of the second condition; the two corresponding CRP cases were found to be univariate outliers. The average of the CRP values of all participants in the same condition and in the same condition order was calculated and substituted for those values. Any data that were not normally distributed were analyzed using methods appropriate for non-normally distributed data.

Data Analysis

In order to determine if TIB and TST were extended, t-test comparisons were completed on the actigraphic variables. Comparisons were made using t-tests (comparing habitual sleep vs sleep extension conditions). Effect sizes (Hedges' g) are also reported in all statistical analysis tables.


#
#
#

Results

Baseline Measures

[Table 2] displays the baseline data for the two time periods. Reported sleepiness was significantly lower after the second baseline (2.70 ± 2.06) compared with the first baseline (4.20 ± 2.94; p = 0.026). No other significant baseline differences were found.

Table 2

Baseline comparisons for all variables to confirm control conditions (n = 10).

Condition

M ± SD

t-value

p-value

Hedges' g

Actigraphic factors

TIB (min)

Baseline 1

486.59 ± 32.96

-0.707

00.497

-0.204

Baseline 2

493.24 ± 32.12

TST (min)

Baseline 1

428.62 ± 29.64

-0.014

0.989

-0.004

Baseline 2

428.76 ± 33.93

Sleep onset latency (min)

Baseline 1

6.03 ± 4.84

-0.988

0.349

-0.286

Baseline 2

7.15 ± 3.47

Sleep efficiency (%)

Baseline 1

88.08 ± 2.33

1.040

0.326

0.300

Baseline 2

86.90 ± 3.54

Wake after sleep onset (min)

Baseline 1

47.55 ± 15.52

-1.201

.260

-0.347

Baseline 2

52.25 ± 19.24

Awakenings (#)

Baseline 1

23.75 ± 4.90

-0.535

0.606

-0.154

Baseline 2

24.30 ± 4.55

Fragmentation (%)

Baseline 1

26.87 ± 4.39

1.345

0.211

0.389

Baseline 2

25.65 ± 5.30

Psychological Factors

Hamilton Anxiety Scale

Baseline 1

1.70 ± 1.70

**

0.438

-0.104

Baseline 2

1.90 ± 1.97

Hamilton Depression Scale

Baseline 1

1.00 ± 0.943

**

0.399

-0.259

Baseline 2

1.60 ± 1.84

BDI-II

Baseline 1

1.40 ± 1.35

**

0.172

0.403

Baseline 2

0.600 ± 1.26

STAI

Baseline 1

26.90 ± 5.24

**

0.351

0.287

Baseline 2

24.70 ± 4.35

Mood

Baseline 1

0.886 ± 1.01

**

0.137

0.322

Baseline 2

0.593 ± 0.948

ESS

Baseline 1

4.20 ± 2.94

**

0.026*

0.700

Baseline 2

2.70 ± 2.06

MAF

Baseline 1

4.91 ± 5.28

**

0.345

0.330

Baseline 2

3.10 ± 3.41

Physiological factors

Systolic BP (mm Hg)

Baseline 1

126.50 ± 13.43

-0.451

0.663

-0.130

Baseline 2

127.93 ± 15.93

Diastolic BP (mm Hg)

Baseline 1

74.17 ± 7.85

0.490

0.636

0.142

Baseline 2

72.83 ± 10.03

Heart rate

Baseline 1

66.62 ± 9.26

-0.926

0.189

-0.268

Baseline 2

68.47 ± 9.56

Abbreviations: BP, blood pressure; ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale; MAF, Multidimensional Assessment of Fatigue; TIB, time in bed; TST, total sleep time.


Notes: *p < 0.05; **Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test performed.



#

Sleep Extension and Manipulation Check

[Figure 1 (a) and (b)] depicts TIB and TST data, respectively. Time in bed was significantly longer during the sleep extension treatment (559.8 ± 36.9 min) compared with the habitual sleep treatment (478.2 ± 19.7 min), with a mean increase of 81.63 ± 33.11 min. Total sleep time was significantly longer in the sleep extension treatment (486.50 ± 40.98 min) compared with the habitual sleep treatment (420.17 ± 25.42 min), with a mean increase of 66.33 ± 28.64 min ([Fig. 2]). There were no detectable differences between participants who advanced their bedtimes, delayed wake times, or both. There were no patterns based on group order.

Zoom Image
Fig. 1 (a) Figure depicts time in bed (TIB), as measured in minutes, for each condition. Gray line represents condition order of habitual sleep (HS) followed by sleep extension (SE; n = 6); black line represents condition order of SE followed by HS (n = 4). Figure 1(b) depicts total sleep time (TST), as measured in minutes, for each condition. Gray line represents condition order of SE followed by HS (n = 4). Error bars represent standard deviation.
Zoom Image
Fig. 2 Figure depicts time in bed and total sleep time, as measured in minutes, for each condition. Error bars represent standard deviation. Asterisk (*) indicates p < 0.001.

#

Actigraphic Sleep Variables

Actigraphic sleep data are presented in [Table 3]. Participants experienced significantly more awakenings (an average of 5.23 more awakenings) during the sleep extension treatment compared to the habitual sleep treatment (t(9) = -4.062, p = 0.003). Wake after sleep onset also increased significantly (13.79 ± 12.72 min) during the sleep extension compared with habitual sleep treatment, (t(9) = -3.254, p = 0.010). No other significant treatment effects were observed for actigraphic sleep.

Table 3

Actigraphic sleep data group comparisons.

Condition

M ± SD

t-value

p-value

Hedges g

TIB (min)

HS

478.24 ± 19.70

-7.797**

0.000027

-2.523

SE

559.87 ± 36.88

TST (min)

HS

420.17 ± 25.42

-7.325**

0.000044

-1.778

SE

486.50 ± 40.98

Sleep onset latency (min)

HS

6.37 ± 3.39

-1.027

0.331

-0.294

SE

7.32 ± 2.44

Sleep efficiency (%)

HS

87.86 ± 3.07

1.242

0.246

0.321

SE

86.82 ± 2.82

Awakenings (#)

HS

22.49 ± 4.68

-4.062**

0.003

-1.073

SE

27.72 ± 4.22

HS

28.18 ± 6.36

-0.717

0.492

-0.256

Fragmentation (%)

SE

29.68 ± 4.11

HS

46.08 ± 12.60

-3.254*

0.010

-0.960

Wake after sleep onset (min)

SE

59.87 ± 13.64

Abbreviations: HS, habitual sleep; SE, sleep extension; TIB, time in bed; TST, total sleep time.


Notes: *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01.



#

Cognitive Variables

No significant treatment differences were found for Stroop Interference scores, PVT data, or Trail Making times, or Stanford-Binet 5 test scores ([Table 4]).

Table 4

Cognitive, mood, and psychological assessments.

Condition

M ± SD

Test Statistic

p-value

Hedges g*

Stroop interference (sec)

BL

37.48 ± 11.96

HS

33.35 ± 7.10

0.211 (F)

0.651

0.225

SE

35.29 ± 9.25

PVT mean response (log10)

BL

2.45 ± 0.048

HS

2.45 ± 0.053

0.013 (F)

0.910

0.000

SE

2.45 ± 0.064

PVT median response (log10)

BL

2.44 ± 0.050

HS

2.44 ± 0.050

0.028 (F)

0.869

0.000

SE

2.44 ± 0.062

PVT fastest 10% (log10)

BL

2.34 ± 0.036

HS

2.35 ± 0.036

0.121 (F)

0.732

-0.242

SE

2.34 ± 0.043

Trail making test A (sec)

BL

30.49 ± 11.83

HS

29.60 ± 14.09

0.352 (F)

0.560

-0.254

SE

26.35 ± 10.08

Trail making test B (sec)

BL

65.00 ± 22.49

HS

75.93 ± 62.20

0.140 (F)

0.713

-0.160

SE

67.32 ± 38.21

Stanford-Binet 5 nonverbal

BL

19.10 ± 2.33

HS

18.40 ± 2.55

0.114 (F)

0.739

0.075

SE

18.60 ± 2.58

Stanford-Binet 5 verbal

BL

19.30 ± 2.45

HS

20.58 ± 2.47

0.635 (F)

0.436

-0.341

SE

19.80 ± 1.87

Hamilton Anxiety Scale

BL (1&2)

1.80 ± 1.62

HS

2.22 ± 3.35

0.149 (F)

0.704

.451

SE

2.70 ± 2.06

Hamilton Depression Scale

BL (1&2)

1.30 ± 1.01

HS

1.52 ± 2.01

0.595 (F)

0.450

.227

SE

2.20 ± 1.93

Beck Depression Inventory-II

HS

1.23 ± 1.13

7.50 (Z)

0.527a

0.131

SE

1.10 ± 1.25

Mood scores

HS

0.729 ± 1.04

13.00 (Z)

0.484a

0.212

SE

0.592 ± 0.806

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

HS

25.13 ± 5.50

18.00 (Z)

0.594a

0.124

SE

25.34 ± 4.32

Epworth Sleepiness Scale

HS

3.67 ± 2.76

8.50

0.182a

0.412

SE

2.43 ± 1.92

Multidimensional Assessment of Fatigue

HS

5.15 ± 4.93

13.00

0.484a

0.320

SE

3.76 ± 4.02

Abbreviations: BL, Baseline; HS, habitual sleep; SE, sleep extension.


Notes: *Hedges g comparisons between habitual sleep and sleep extension; aRelated-Samples Wilcoxon Signed Rank Test.



#

Psychological Variables

Psychological data are displayed in [Table 4]. No significant treatment differences were found for depression (Hamilton Depression Scale) or anxiety (Hamilton Anxiety Scale) as assessed by clinical psychology graduate students.

[Table 4] displays the self-reported psychological data. Related-samples Wilcoxon Signed Rank tests were conducted on the subjective depression, mood, anxiety, sleepiness, and fatigue assessments since the data were not normally distributed. No significant treatment differences were found for depressed mood scores (BDI-II), Likert-rated mood scores, or anxiety scores (STAI). No significant treatment differences were found for sleepiness (ESS) or fatigue (MAF). Effect sizes indicated moderate effects for decreased subjective sleepiness after the sleep extension treatment compared to the habitual sleep treatment.


#

Physiological Data

[Table 5] displays the physiological data. No significant treatment differences were found for measures of inflammation (log-transformed CRP), glucose, triglycerides, or cholesterol (HDL and LDL; see [Fig. 3]).

Zoom Image
Fig. 3 Figure depicts lipid panel results; Hedges' g effect size (-0.490) indicated a moderate increase in triglycerides after the sleep extension condition. No significant differences seen in HDL (high-density lipoprotein) or LDL (low-density lipoprotein). Error bars represent standard deviation.
Table 5

Physiological measures assessed from blood draw and blood pressure cuff.

Measure

Condition

M ± SD

Test Statistic

p-value

Hedges g*

CRP (log(10), mg/L)

HS

0.126 ± 0.364

0.394 (t)

0.349a

-0.030

SE

0.134 ± 0.230

Glucose (mg/dl)

HS

93.90 ± 5.88

-0.283 (t)

0.390a

-0.159

SE

94.70 ± 6.73

Trig (mg/dl)

HS

73.90 ± 14.27

-1.055 (t)

0.153a

-0.490

SE

81.50 ± 17.75

HDL (mg/dl)

HS

74.60 ± 20.67

-0.076 (t)

0.470a

-0.062

SE

75.20 ± 13.80

LDL (mg/dl)

HS

107.00 ± 48.24

-0.377 (t)

0.355a

-0.214

SE

115.50 ± 52.39

Systolic BP (mm Hg)

BL (1 & 2)

127.22 ± 13.85

0.040 (F)

0.844

-0.086

HS

125.82 ± 13.49

SE

124.68 ± 11.89

Diastolic BP (mm Hg)

BL (1&2)

73.50 ± 7.91

0.329 (F)

0.573

-0.246

HS

72.19 ± 6.32

SE

70.53 ± 6.63

Heart Rate

BL (1&2)

67.54 ± 8.86

0.031 (F)

0.861

0.076

HS

68.89 ± 8.69

SE

69.67 ± 10.82

Abbreviations: BP, blood pressure, BP; CRP, C-reactive protein; HS, habitual sleep; HDL, high-density lipoprotein; LDL, low-density lipoprotein; SE, sleep extension; Trig, triglycerides.


Notes: *Hedges g reported for habitual sleep vs. sleep extension; aIndependent Samples t-Test.


No significant treatment differences were found in systolic blood pressure, diastolic blood pressure, or heart rate.


#
#

Discussion

These relatively healthy, middle-aged-to-older average-duration sleepers were able to increase their TIB and TST sleep duration by an average of 81.63 ± 33.11 min and 66.33 ± 28.64 min, respectively, for 3 weeks in the sleep extension treatment, whereas TIB and TST in the habitual sleep treatment were similar to baseline levels. However, compared with the habitual sleep treatment, the sleep extension treatment did not significantly affect any of the dependent variables, and associated effect sizes were generally small.

Average adherence to the TIB extension was less than the target of 120 min. Only 2 of the 10 participants had TIB extension of ≥ 120 min, and 5 more participants had an average TIB increase of over 60 min.

Time in bed extension has been difficult to accomplish, as the average percentage of the targeted TIB extension in the present study (68%) was similar to that reported previously by our group[32] and other researchers (see Baron et al.[33] and Henst et al.[55] meta-analyses). In the weekly visits with research staff, participants were reminded to try to adhere to the TIB extension target, but since the ability to adhere to this target was a dependent variable, these reminders were mild. Anecdotally, participants indicated that they found it difficult to extend their TIB by the requested amount.

One distinct difference between experimental extension of sleep in healthy individuals and long sleepers is sleep pressure and perceived sleep need. It is possible that individuals who are obtaining healthy amounts of sleep do not experience the same level of sleep pressure, making it more difficult to achieve extra sleep when it is not needed. This particular sleep pressure factor makes experimental sleep extension much more complicated, as it becomes harder for participants to achieve their TST goals. A potentially more successful future approach may be to gradually increase TIB, perhaps based on the participants' sleep efficiency, analogous to the process of slightly and incrementally increasing TIB following the initial sleep restriction in cognitive behavioral treatment for insomnia.[56]

Other than the increase in TST, the TIB extension had minimal effect on sleep. Although wake after sleep onset and number of awakenings increased significantly in the sleep extension treatment compared to the habitual sleep treatment, the amount of wake time after sleep onset (46.08 ± 12.60 and 59.87 ± 13.64 min, respectively) and number of awakenings (22.49 ± 4.68 and 27.72 ± 4.22, respectively) per hour of TIB were similar for the sleep extension and habitual sleep treatments. These data and the lack of significant difference in sleep efficiency between treatments is contrary to the notion that spending excessive TIB could lead to more sleep fragmentation, as observed, for example, in individuals with insomnia. Nonetheless, these results are consistent with previous work by our team and others (e.g., increased awakenings and wake after sleep onset[30] [34]; sleep efficiency did not significantly change despite the increased wakefulness).[34]

Neither significant positive nor negative effects of sleep extension were found for any of the variables assessed. The lack of positive effects contrasts with recent studies of individuals with short sleep duration, which have found beneficial effects of sleep extension on cognition, mood, appetite, blood pressure, and prediabetes (see meta-analysis by Baron et al.[33]). However, key differences between the present study and these other studies include that participants in the present study had average age-related sleep duration and relatively good health. The majority of recent experimental sleep extension studies target short sleepers with various health problems, using sleep extension as an intervention to increase sleep duration.[33] However, for the present study, levels of blood pressure (systolic 125.82 ± 13.49 Hg, diastolic 72.19 ± 6.32 Hg), HDL (74.60 ± 20.67 mg/dl), sleepiness (3.67 ± 2.76), state anxiety (25.13 ± 5.50), and depressed mood (1.23 ± 1.13) are considered quite healthy for older adults, close to the ceiling/floor of optimal health such that significant improvements are less likely.[57]

Negative effects of sleep extension were regarded as more likely in the present study, based on our previous research[32] and other experimental sleep extension studies. For example, other studies have found impairments in performance on vigilance tasks after sleep extension (e.g., Taub et al., Taub & Berger[30] [58]). The average extent and duration of TST increase (66.33 ± 28.64 min) might have been insufficient to produce the same level of grogginess and delay found in previous studies.

Although not significant, we anticipated that the triglycerides would increase after sleep extension compared to habitual sleep, which would have been consistent with other evidence that long sleep is associated with higher levels of triglycerides in women.[59] Total cholesterol levels were not significantly different between treatments, a result which was inconsistent with previous epidemiological studies examining long sleep in older adults.[60]

The current study had many significant strengths. To our knowledge, this is the first study to experimentally examine the physiological, cognitive, and psychological effects of chronic increases in sleep duration in middle-aged and older adults. The study was the first to examine sleep extension in healthy, older sleepers across such a comprehensive number of health factors through the use of a randomized, cross-over study design. The study demonstrated feasibility for middle-age and older adults to extend their sleep by extending their TIB attempting to sleep. The baseline differences in the various measures were not significant, suggesting there were not carry-over effects with a one-week washout period. These findings are important for future cross-over studies, as they provide evidence that a one-week recovery is sufficient to reduce or eliminate carry-over effects in an experimental sleep extension study.

The study also had several weaknesses. Although mitigated somewhat by a cross-over design, the sample size was small. Additionally, sleep extension was modest, and the duration of the intervention was short. More intense sleep extension (e.g., more than 2 hours) and intervention duration (e.g., more than 3 weeks) may produce different results. Participants were unable to extend their TIB or sleep duration to the extent that was planned, confirming the difficulty in voluntary sleep extension. Finally, due to the small number of participants, the impact of important social determinants (e.g., socioeconomic status) could not be evaluated and affects the generalizability of the results.

Future randomized controlled trials should examine extended sleep over a longer period of time (i.e., more than 3 weeks) and/or longer duration of TIB (i.e., more than 2 hours per night) in order to determine to what extent older adults with average sleep duration might experience negative effects following extension of sleep duration. Considering that sleep extension is difficult for healthy habitual sleepers, a greater degree of TIB extension might require extensive efforts involving gradual extension and greater troubleshooting to ensure adherence to extension protocols. However, it is acknowledged that even a longer-duration study may still not directly compare to individuals who sleep longer over several years or even decades, as increases in sleep would be acute and impact rapidly-reversible outcomes in experimental sleep extension conditions. If longer-duration studies are performed and significant effects are found, it would be imperative to conduct follow-ups (e.g., 3 months, 6 months, etc.) after study completion to determine if those significant changes are sustained over time. Finally, researchers conducting experimental sleep extension studies should be careful about comparing conclusions to epidemiological studies of long sleepers and increased health risks, as long sleepers tend to present this sleep phenotype over a significant amount of time.


#
#

Conflict of Interests

The authors have no conflict of interests to declare.

Disclosure

The author reports no conflicts of interest in this work.


  • References

  • 1 Kripke DF, Garfinkel L, Wingard DL, Klauber MR, Marler MR. Mortality associated with sleep duration and insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59 (02) 131-136
  • 2 Fan M, Sun D, Zhou T. et al. Sleep patterns, genetic susceptibility, and incident cardiovascular disease: a prospective study of 385 292 UK biobank participants. Eur Heart J 2020; 41 (11) 1182-1189
  • 3 Jike M, Itani O, Watanabe N, Buysse DJ, Kaneita Y. Long sleep duration and health outcomes: A systematic review, meta-analysis and meta-regression. Sleep Med Rev 2018; 39: 25-36
  • 4 Dong L, Xie Y, Zou X. Association between sleep duration and depression in US adults: A cross-sectional study. J Affect Disord 2022; 296: 183-188
  • 5 Ma Y, Liang L, Zheng F, Shi L, Zhong B, Xie W. Association between sleep duration and cognitive decline. JAMA Netw Open 2020; 3 (09) e2013573
  • 6 Guo X, Zheng L, Wang J. et al. Epidemiological evidence for the link between sleep duration and high blood pressure: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med 2013; 14 (04) 324-332
  • 7 Reis C, Dias S, Rodrigues AM. et al. Sleep duration, lifestyles and chronic diseases: a cross-sectional population-based study. Sleep Sci 2018; 11 (04) 217-230
  • 8 Smiley A, King D, Bidulescu A. The association between sleep duration and metabolic syndrome: The NHANES 2013/2014. Nutrients 2019; 11 (11) 2582
  • 9 Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, Cappuccio FP. et al. A prospective study of change in sleep duration: associations with mortality in the Whitehall II cohort. Sleep 2007; 30 (12) 1659-1666
  • 10 Cohen-Mansfield J, Perach R. Sleep duration, nap habits, and mortality in older persons. Sleep 2012; 35 (07) 1003-1009
  • 11 Youngstedt SD, Kripke DF. Long sleep and mortality: rationale for sleep restriction. Sleep Med Rev 2004; 8 (03) 159-174
  • 12 Stamatakis KA, Punjabi NM. Long sleep duration: a risk to health or a marker of risk?. Sleep Med Rev 2007; 11 (05) 337-339
  • 13 Duggan KA, Reynolds CA, Kern ML, Friedman HS. Childhood sleep duration and lifelong mortality risk. Health Psychol 2014; 33 (10) 1195-1203
  • 14 Mesas AE, López-García E, León-Muñoz LM, Guallar-Castillón P, Rodríguez-Artalejo F. Sleep duration and mortality according to health status in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2010; 58 (10) 1870-1877
  • 15 Knutson KL, Turek FW. The U-shaped association between sleep and health: the 2 peaks do not mean the same thing. Sleep 2006; 29 (07) 878-879
  • 16 Hamburg NM, McMackin CJ, Huang AL. et al. Physical inactivity rapidly induces insulin resistance and microvascular dysfunction in healthy volunteers. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 2007; 27 (12) 2650-2656
  • 17 Schneider SM, Robergs RA, Amorim FT, de Serna DG, Duran-Valdez EE, Schade DS. Impaired orthostatic response in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus after 48 hours of bed rest. Endocr Pract 2009; 15 (02) 104-110
  • 18 Matthews CE, George SM, Moore SC. et al. Amount of time spent in sedentary behaviors and cause-specific mortality in US adults. Am J Clin Nutr 2012; 95 (02) 437-445
  • 19 Buman MP, Kline CE, Youngstedt SD, Phillips B, Tulio de Mello M, Hirshkowitz M. Sitting and television viewing: novel risk factors for sleep disturbance and apnea risk? results from the 2013 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Poll. Chest 2015; 147 (03) 728-734
  • 20 Stranges S, Dorn JM, Shipley MJ. et al. Correlates of short and long sleep duration: a cross-cultural comparison between the United Kingdom and the United States: the Whitehall II Study and the Western New York Health Study. Am J Epidemiol 2008; 168 (12) 1353-1364
  • 21 Stepanski EJ. The effect of sleep fragmentation on daytime function. Sleep 2002; 25 (03) 268-276
  • 22 Patel SR, Zhu X, Storfer-Isser A. et al. Sleep duration and biomarkers of inflammation. Sleep 2009; 32 (02) 200-204
  • 23 Veler H. Sleep and inflammation. Sleep Med Clin 2023; 18 (02) 213-218
  • 24 Bhattacharjee Y. Psychiatric research. Is internal timing key to mental health?. Science 2007; 317 (5844) 1488-1490
  • 25 Olanrewaju HA, Miller WW, Maslin WR, Collier SD, Purswell JL, Branton SL. Influence of light sources and photoperiod on growth performance, carcass characteristics, and health indices of broilers grown to heavy weights. Poult Sci 2018; 97 (04) 1109-1116
  • 26 Winer JR, Deters KD, Kennedy G. et al. Association of short and long sleep duration with amyloid-β burden and cognition in aging. JAMA Neurol 2021; 78 (10) 1187-1196
  • 27 Ding G, Li J, Lian Z. Both short and long sleep durations are associated with cognitive impairment among community-dwelling Chinese older adults. Medicine (Baltimore) 2020; 99 (13) e19667
  • 28 Hua J, Zhuang S, Shen Y, Tang X, Sun H, Fang Q. Exploring the bidirectional associations between short or long sleep duration and lower cognitive function: A 7-year cohort study in China. Front Aging Neurosci 2021; 13: 727763
  • 29 van Oostrom SH, Nooyens ACJ, van Boxtel MPJ, Verschuren WMM. Long sleep duration is associated with lower cognitive function among middle-age adults - the Doetinchem Cohort Study. Sleep Med 2018; 41: 78-85
  • 30 Taub JM, Globus GG, Phoebus E, Drury R. Extended sleep and performance. Nature 1971; 233 (5315) 142-143
  • 31 Taub JM, Berger RJ. Performance and mood following variations in the length and timing of sleep. Psychophysiology 1973; 10 (06) 559-570
  • 32 Reynold AM, Bowles ER, Saxena A, Fayad R, Youngstedt SD. Negative effects of time in bed extension: A pilot study. J Sleep Med Disord 2014; 1 (01) 1002
  • 33 Baron KG, Duffecy J, Reutrakul S. et al. Behavioral interventions to extend sleep duration: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev 2021; 60: 101532
  • 34 Gonzales JU, Clark C, Anderson T. Effect of five nights of sleep extension on peripheral vascular function: a randomized crossover investigation into long sleep duration. Sleep Med 2022; 90: 145-152
  • 35 Clark C, Rivas E, Gonzales JU. Six nights of sleep extension increases regional cerebral oxygenation without modifying cognitive performance at rest or following acute aerobic exercise. J Sleep Res 2022; 31 (05) e13582
  • 36 Youngstedt SD, Kline CE, Zielinski MR. et al. Tolerance of chronic 90-minute time-in-bed restriction in older long sleepers. Sleep 2009; 32 (11) 1467-1479
  • 37 Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, Vitiello MV. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 2004; 27 (07) 1255-1273
  • 38 Youngstedt SD, Goff EE, Reynolds AM. et al. Has adult sleep duration declined over the last 50+ years?. Sleep Med Rev 2016; 28: 69-85
  • 39 Miner B, Kryger MH. Sleep in the aging population. Sleep Med Clin 2020; 15 (02) 311-318
  • 40 Cole RJ, Kripke DF, Gruen W, Mullaney DJ, Gillin JC. Automatic sleep/wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep 1992; 15 (05) 461-469
  • 41 Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23 (01) 56-62
  • 42 Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br J Med Psychol 1959; 32 (01) 50-55
  • 43 Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation; 1996
  • 44 Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography. 2nd ed. Consulting Psychologists Press;; 1989
  • 45 Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 1991; 14 (06) 540-545
  • 46 Belza BL, Henke CJ, Yelin EH, Epstein WV, Gilliss CL. Correlates of fatigue in older adults with rheumatoid arthritis. Nurs Res 1993; 42 (02) 93-99
  • 47 Roid GH. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales. 5th ed. (SB:V. Riverside Publishing; 2003
  • 48 Drummond SP, Bischoff-Grethe A, Dinges DF, Ayalon L, Mednick SC, Meloy MJ. The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep 2005; 28 (09) 1059-1068
  • 49 Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935; 18: 643-662
  • 50 Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 2006; 13 (01) 62-79
  • 51 Valentijn SAM, van Boxtel MP, van Hooren SA. et al. Change in sensory functioning predicts change in cognitive functioning: results from a 6-year follow-up in the maastricht aging study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53 (03) 374-380
  • 52 Oosterman JM, Vogels RLC, van Harten B. et al. Assessing mental flexibility: neuroanatomical and neuropsychological correlates of the Trail Making Test in elderly people. Clin Neuropsychol 2010; 24 (02) 203-219
  • 53 Takahashi H, Yoshika M, Yokoi T. Validation of Omron RS8, RS6, and RS3 home blood pressure monitoring devices, in accordance with the European Society of Hypertension International Protocol revision 2010. Vasc Health Risk Manag 2013; 9: 265-272
  • 54 O'Connor MF, Irwin MR. Links between behavioral factors and inflammation. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2010; 87 (04) 479-482
  • 55 Henst RHP, Pienaar PR, Roden LC, Rae DE. The effects of sleep extension on cardiometabolic risk factors: A systematic review. J Sleep Res 2019; 28 (06) e12865
  • 56 Cunnington D, Junge MF, Fernando AT. Insomnia: prevalence, consequences and effective treatment. Med J Aust 2013; 199 (08) S36-S40
  • 57 Youngstedt SD. Ceiling and floor effects in sleep research. Sleep Med Rev 2003; 7 (04) 351-365
  • 58 Taub JM, Berger RJ. Extended sleep and performance: The Rip Van Winkle effect. Psychon Sci 1969; 16 (04) 204-205
  • 59 Kaneita Y, Uchiyama M, Yoshiike N, Ohida T. Associations of usual sleep duration with serum lipid and lipoprotein levels. Sleep 2008; 31 (05) 645-652
  • 60 van den Berg JF, Miedema HME, Tulen JHM. et al. Long sleep duration is associated with serum cholesterol in the elderly: the Rotterdam Study. Psychosom Med 2008; 70 (09) 1005-1011

Address for correspondence

Alexandria M. Reynolds

Publication History

Received: 30 June 2023

Accepted: 28 November 2023

Article published online:
13 June 2024

© 2024. Brazilian Sleep Association. This is an open access article published by Thieme under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonDerivative-NonCommercial License, permitting copying and reproduction so long as the original work is given appropriate credit. Contents may not be used for commercial purposes, or adapted, remixed, transformed or built upon. (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/)

Thieme Revinter Publicações Ltda.
Rua do Matoso 170, Rio de Janeiro, RJ, CEP 20270-135, Brazil

  • References

  • 1 Kripke DF, Garfinkel L, Wingard DL, Klauber MR, Marler MR. Mortality associated with sleep duration and insomnia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2002; 59 (02) 131-136
  • 2 Fan M, Sun D, Zhou T. et al. Sleep patterns, genetic susceptibility, and incident cardiovascular disease: a prospective study of 385 292 UK biobank participants. Eur Heart J 2020; 41 (11) 1182-1189
  • 3 Jike M, Itani O, Watanabe N, Buysse DJ, Kaneita Y. Long sleep duration and health outcomes: A systematic review, meta-analysis and meta-regression. Sleep Med Rev 2018; 39: 25-36
  • 4 Dong L, Xie Y, Zou X. Association between sleep duration and depression in US adults: A cross-sectional study. J Affect Disord 2022; 296: 183-188
  • 5 Ma Y, Liang L, Zheng F, Shi L, Zhong B, Xie W. Association between sleep duration and cognitive decline. JAMA Netw Open 2020; 3 (09) e2013573
  • 6 Guo X, Zheng L, Wang J. et al. Epidemiological evidence for the link between sleep duration and high blood pressure: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med 2013; 14 (04) 324-332
  • 7 Reis C, Dias S, Rodrigues AM. et al. Sleep duration, lifestyles and chronic diseases: a cross-sectional population-based study. Sleep Sci 2018; 11 (04) 217-230
  • 8 Smiley A, King D, Bidulescu A. The association between sleep duration and metabolic syndrome: The NHANES 2013/2014. Nutrients 2019; 11 (11) 2582
  • 9 Ferrie JE, Shipley MJ, Cappuccio FP. et al. A prospective study of change in sleep duration: associations with mortality in the Whitehall II cohort. Sleep 2007; 30 (12) 1659-1666
  • 10 Cohen-Mansfield J, Perach R. Sleep duration, nap habits, and mortality in older persons. Sleep 2012; 35 (07) 1003-1009
  • 11 Youngstedt SD, Kripke DF. Long sleep and mortality: rationale for sleep restriction. Sleep Med Rev 2004; 8 (03) 159-174
  • 12 Stamatakis KA, Punjabi NM. Long sleep duration: a risk to health or a marker of risk?. Sleep Med Rev 2007; 11 (05) 337-339
  • 13 Duggan KA, Reynolds CA, Kern ML, Friedman HS. Childhood sleep duration and lifelong mortality risk. Health Psychol 2014; 33 (10) 1195-1203
  • 14 Mesas AE, López-García E, León-Muñoz LM, Guallar-Castillón P, Rodríguez-Artalejo F. Sleep duration and mortality according to health status in older adults. J Am Geriatr Soc 2010; 58 (10) 1870-1877
  • 15 Knutson KL, Turek FW. The U-shaped association between sleep and health: the 2 peaks do not mean the same thing. Sleep 2006; 29 (07) 878-879
  • 16 Hamburg NM, McMackin CJ, Huang AL. et al. Physical inactivity rapidly induces insulin resistance and microvascular dysfunction in healthy volunteers. Arterioscler Thromb Vasc Biol 2007; 27 (12) 2650-2656
  • 17 Schneider SM, Robergs RA, Amorim FT, de Serna DG, Duran-Valdez EE, Schade DS. Impaired orthostatic response in patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus after 48 hours of bed rest. Endocr Pract 2009; 15 (02) 104-110
  • 18 Matthews CE, George SM, Moore SC. et al. Amount of time spent in sedentary behaviors and cause-specific mortality in US adults. Am J Clin Nutr 2012; 95 (02) 437-445
  • 19 Buman MP, Kline CE, Youngstedt SD, Phillips B, Tulio de Mello M, Hirshkowitz M. Sitting and television viewing: novel risk factors for sleep disturbance and apnea risk? results from the 2013 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America Poll. Chest 2015; 147 (03) 728-734
  • 20 Stranges S, Dorn JM, Shipley MJ. et al. Correlates of short and long sleep duration: a cross-cultural comparison between the United Kingdom and the United States: the Whitehall II Study and the Western New York Health Study. Am J Epidemiol 2008; 168 (12) 1353-1364
  • 21 Stepanski EJ. The effect of sleep fragmentation on daytime function. Sleep 2002; 25 (03) 268-276
  • 22 Patel SR, Zhu X, Storfer-Isser A. et al. Sleep duration and biomarkers of inflammation. Sleep 2009; 32 (02) 200-204
  • 23 Veler H. Sleep and inflammation. Sleep Med Clin 2023; 18 (02) 213-218
  • 24 Bhattacharjee Y. Psychiatric research. Is internal timing key to mental health?. Science 2007; 317 (5844) 1488-1490
  • 25 Olanrewaju HA, Miller WW, Maslin WR, Collier SD, Purswell JL, Branton SL. Influence of light sources and photoperiod on growth performance, carcass characteristics, and health indices of broilers grown to heavy weights. Poult Sci 2018; 97 (04) 1109-1116
  • 26 Winer JR, Deters KD, Kennedy G. et al. Association of short and long sleep duration with amyloid-β burden and cognition in aging. JAMA Neurol 2021; 78 (10) 1187-1196
  • 27 Ding G, Li J, Lian Z. Both short and long sleep durations are associated with cognitive impairment among community-dwelling Chinese older adults. Medicine (Baltimore) 2020; 99 (13) e19667
  • 28 Hua J, Zhuang S, Shen Y, Tang X, Sun H, Fang Q. Exploring the bidirectional associations between short or long sleep duration and lower cognitive function: A 7-year cohort study in China. Front Aging Neurosci 2021; 13: 727763
  • 29 van Oostrom SH, Nooyens ACJ, van Boxtel MPJ, Verschuren WMM. Long sleep duration is associated with lower cognitive function among middle-age adults - the Doetinchem Cohort Study. Sleep Med 2018; 41: 78-85
  • 30 Taub JM, Globus GG, Phoebus E, Drury R. Extended sleep and performance. Nature 1971; 233 (5315) 142-143
  • 31 Taub JM, Berger RJ. Performance and mood following variations in the length and timing of sleep. Psychophysiology 1973; 10 (06) 559-570
  • 32 Reynold AM, Bowles ER, Saxena A, Fayad R, Youngstedt SD. Negative effects of time in bed extension: A pilot study. J Sleep Med Disord 2014; 1 (01) 1002
  • 33 Baron KG, Duffecy J, Reutrakul S. et al. Behavioral interventions to extend sleep duration: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Med Rev 2021; 60: 101532
  • 34 Gonzales JU, Clark C, Anderson T. Effect of five nights of sleep extension on peripheral vascular function: a randomized crossover investigation into long sleep duration. Sleep Med 2022; 90: 145-152
  • 35 Clark C, Rivas E, Gonzales JU. Six nights of sleep extension increases regional cerebral oxygenation without modifying cognitive performance at rest or following acute aerobic exercise. J Sleep Res 2022; 31 (05) e13582
  • 36 Youngstedt SD, Kline CE, Zielinski MR. et al. Tolerance of chronic 90-minute time-in-bed restriction in older long sleepers. Sleep 2009; 32 (11) 1467-1479
  • 37 Ohayon MM, Carskadon MA, Guilleminault C, Vitiello MV. Meta-analysis of quantitative sleep parameters from childhood to old age in healthy individuals: developing normative sleep values across the human lifespan. Sleep 2004; 27 (07) 1255-1273
  • 38 Youngstedt SD, Goff EE, Reynolds AM. et al. Has adult sleep duration declined over the last 50+ years?. Sleep Med Rev 2016; 28: 69-85
  • 39 Miner B, Kryger MH. Sleep in the aging population. Sleep Med Clin 2020; 15 (02) 311-318
  • 40 Cole RJ, Kripke DF, Gruen W, Mullaney DJ, Gillin JC. Automatic sleep/wake identification from wrist activity. Sleep 1992; 15 (05) 461-469
  • 41 Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1960; 23 (01) 56-62
  • 42 Hamilton M. The assessment of anxiety states by rating. Br J Med Psychol 1959; 32 (01) 50-55
  • 43 Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II. Psychological Corporation; 1996
  • 44 Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography. 2nd ed. Consulting Psychologists Press;; 1989
  • 45 Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 1991; 14 (06) 540-545
  • 46 Belza BL, Henke CJ, Yelin EH, Epstein WV, Gilliss CL. Correlates of fatigue in older adults with rheumatoid arthritis. Nurs Res 1993; 42 (02) 93-99
  • 47 Roid GH. Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales. 5th ed. (SB:V. Riverside Publishing; 2003
  • 48 Drummond SP, Bischoff-Grethe A, Dinges DF, Ayalon L, Mednick SC, Meloy MJ. The neural basis of the psychomotor vigilance task. Sleep 2005; 28 (09) 1059-1068
  • 49 Stroop JR. Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 1935; 18: 643-662
  • 50 Van der Elst W, Van Boxtel MP, Van Breukelen GJ, Jolles J. The Stroop color-word test: influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 2006; 13 (01) 62-79
  • 51 Valentijn SAM, van Boxtel MP, van Hooren SA. et al. Change in sensory functioning predicts change in cognitive functioning: results from a 6-year follow-up in the maastricht aging study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53 (03) 374-380
  • 52 Oosterman JM, Vogels RLC, van Harten B. et al. Assessing mental flexibility: neuroanatomical and neuropsychological correlates of the Trail Making Test in elderly people. Clin Neuropsychol 2010; 24 (02) 203-219
  • 53 Takahashi H, Yoshika M, Yokoi T. Validation of Omron RS8, RS6, and RS3 home blood pressure monitoring devices, in accordance with the European Society of Hypertension International Protocol revision 2010. Vasc Health Risk Manag 2013; 9: 265-272
  • 54 O'Connor MF, Irwin MR. Links between behavioral factors and inflammation. Clin Pharmacol Ther 2010; 87 (04) 479-482
  • 55 Henst RHP, Pienaar PR, Roden LC, Rae DE. The effects of sleep extension on cardiometabolic risk factors: A systematic review. J Sleep Res 2019; 28 (06) e12865
  • 56 Cunnington D, Junge MF, Fernando AT. Insomnia: prevalence, consequences and effective treatment. Med J Aust 2013; 199 (08) S36-S40
  • 57 Youngstedt SD. Ceiling and floor effects in sleep research. Sleep Med Rev 2003; 7 (04) 351-365
  • 58 Taub JM, Berger RJ. Extended sleep and performance: The Rip Van Winkle effect. Psychon Sci 1969; 16 (04) 204-205
  • 59 Kaneita Y, Uchiyama M, Yoshiike N, Ohida T. Associations of usual sleep duration with serum lipid and lipoprotein levels. Sleep 2008; 31 (05) 645-652
  • 60 van den Berg JF, Miedema HME, Tulen JHM. et al. Long sleep duration is associated with serum cholesterol in the elderly: the Rotterdam Study. Psychosom Med 2008; 70 (09) 1005-1011

Zoom Image
Fig. 1 (a) Figure depicts time in bed (TIB), as measured in minutes, for each condition. Gray line represents condition order of habitual sleep (HS) followed by sleep extension (SE; n = 6); black line represents condition order of SE followed by HS (n = 4). Figure 1(b) depicts total sleep time (TST), as measured in minutes, for each condition. Gray line represents condition order of SE followed by HS (n = 4). Error bars represent standard deviation.
Zoom Image
Fig. 2 Figure depicts time in bed and total sleep time, as measured in minutes, for each condition. Error bars represent standard deviation. Asterisk (*) indicates p < 0.001.
Zoom Image
Fig. 3 Figure depicts lipid panel results; Hedges' g effect size (-0.490) indicated a moderate increase in triglycerides after the sleep extension condition. No significant differences seen in HDL (high-density lipoprotein) or LDL (low-density lipoprotein). Error bars represent standard deviation.