Keywords:
Kurt Goldstein - Neuropsychological Tests - Neurocognitive disorders - Abstract attitude
Palabras-clave:
Kurt Goldstein - Test neuropsicológicos - Déficits neurocognitivos - Actitud abstracta
Clinical neuropsychology seems crazy about numbers: Cognitive abilities have to be
quantified and normed in order to serve as a measure of an individual’s performance
relative to a population. A deviance may relate to a brain damage. An early critic
of such a merely quantitative assessment was the German-American neurologist and psychiatrist
Kurt Goldstein (1878-1965), one of the founders of holistic neurology and neuropsychology
[1],[2]. After having fled Nazi Germany, Goldstein worked in the USA where he published
a remarkable monograph[3] together with the psychologist Martin Scheerer (1900-1961). In this work, the authors
proposed a qualitative test procedure for the assessment of cognitive deficits which they illustrated by
means of a didactic film.
The paradigm used by Goldstein and Scheerer is one among the earliest (neuro)psychological
tests[4], the Block Design Test. It is part of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) and is still being widely used for the assessment of visuospatial construction
in clinical neuropsychology[5]. Subjects are asked to arrange wooden cubes displaying three different types of
color patterns on their sides (plain white, plain red or diagonally divided red and
white, but older versions used different colors) according to a model on paper. The
Gestalt of the target pattern has to be mentally segmented into parts corresponding to the
different sides of the cubes which may then be arranged accordingly.
Visuospatial construction as tested by the Block Design Test is the ability to integrate
sensory input into a spatial representation and to enable an adequate motor response
in extrapersonal space[5]. Historically, visuospatial impairments have been classified under a variety of
terms such as ‘optic ataxia’ or ‘constructive apraxia’[6]. They have proven valuable in the differential diagnosis of neurological conditions
such as dementia[7]. However, the standard use of the Block Design Test (e.g., as part of the WAIS)
in neuropsychology has been criticized, as it scores only the number of correct solutions
and thus discards qualitative aspects of performance[5],[8]. The usefulness of a qualitative analysis of Block Design Test performance of brain
damaged patients has been repeatedly demonstrated[9].
In this respect, Goldstein and Scheerer[3] presented pioneering work. By way of some modifications of the original procedure[4], they categorized different types of errors in patients’ performance, such as size
transformation (the model on paper being larger than the arrangement of blocks), mental
segmentation of the model, and consideration of all relevant features (color and form),
etc. Their test version seems profoundly unconventional in the eyes of psychometrically
oriented neuropsychologists (see [Figure 1]): If an item was not correctly solved ([Figure 1a]), the following forms of graded help were offered: (i) Enlarged model matching the
arrangement of blocks in size, thus no size transformation is demanded ([Figure 1b]). (ii) Original model with grid lines segmenting the model into the different cube
sides ([Figure 1c]). (iii) The same with an enlarged design matching the block arrangement in size.
(iv) Model as an arrangement of wooden blocks ([Figure 1d]). (v) The same as (iv) but with space between the blocks. (vi) Multiple choice among
three models presented built from blocks. In case of success of any of these steps
(i)-(vi), the original task was repeated in order to test the transfer of what had
been learned. The patient’s procedure was recorded in detail, including final block
arrangements, strategy, and patient’s comments and judgments.
Figure 1 Illustration of how graded cues enhanced the diagnosis (according to type and severity)
of disturbed abstract attitude in a brain-damaged subject. The patient was asked to
arrange four wooden cubes. Their sides display patterns (plain blue, plain yellow,
or diagonally divided blue and yellow) and have to be arranged according to a model
presented on paper (Goldstein-Scheerer Cube Test). The patient fails to solve the test item in its standard presentation (a). The
patient does neither profit from a target pattern matching in size (b) nor from a
target pattern with a grid of lines indicating the segmentation of the Gestalt (c;
target pattern in lower left-hand corner added by the authors for illustration purposes).
As the demand to abstract is minimized by giving the correct solution in its most
concrete form, i.e., in the form of correctly arranged wooden cubes as in (d), the
patient is able to reproduce the requested pattern. However, he is unable to transfer
the solution in (d) to the original task (a). A severe disturbance of the abstract
attitude has thus to be diagnosed. (Stills are taken from a film accompanying the
monograph of Goldstein und Scheerer [3] with kind permission of the Rare Book and Manuscript Library at Columbia University,
New York. Figure is adapted from a previous publication [20]).
Goldstein continuously objected to psychometric testing of neurological patients,
since standardization and mere quantification would not reveal the actual impairment
of the individual patient[10]. This does not contradict the fact that Goldstein was probably one of the first
to administer a comprehensive and psychologically elaborated test battery to patients
ever since being head of the Institute for Research into the Aftereffects of Brain Injury in Frankfurt am Main, a clinic for the rehabilitation of brain-damaged First World
War veterans[2]. Goldstein repeatedly criticized the approach of nineteenth century neurology which
he had first adopted as one of Carl Wernicke’s (1848-1905) students. This approach
focused exclusively on whether a task would be solved or not[11]. Goldstein argued that a correct solution could also have been achieved by means
of some compensating strategy. Furthermore, just registering that a solution was incorrect
would not reveal anything about the nature of the patient’s deficit. A qualitative
assessment was thus necessary as a prerequisite for successful rehabilitation. In
addition, it could account for the idea that neuro(psycho)logical symptoms are intrinsically
complex[12],[13] and dependent on the context of both the individual and the situation.
In contrast to nineteenth century mechanistic views of the brain, Goldstein regarded
the brain as a Netzwerk (network)[11], i.e., a systemic whole. Any brain damage would break up and reduce this system’s
performance through destruction of cerebral tissue as well as disconnection effects.
The remaining network would become less effective in holding and counterbalancing
excitation by external stimuli. Hence, the patients’ behavior would become more coarse-grained,
stereotypical, and stimulus-bound. In Goldstein’s terms, patients were forced to retain
a concrete attitude towards the world, whereas intact brains allow us to refrain from immediate action
and to reflect about the given situation, in short, to adopt an abstract (or categorial) attitude.
Goldstein considered the abstract attitude as an essential human ability[10]. He took its disturbance to be a Grundstörung (basic disturbance) resulting from brain damage or from severe mental illnesses such
as schizophrenia[14]. Since the propensity to perform abstractions is anything but monolithic, Goldstein’s
and Scheerer’s[3] qualitative assessment aimed at grasping the severity of disturbance of the abstract attitude in a test of visuospatial construction. The abstract attitude being independent of
stimulus modality, these authors presented another test in the same publication[3], a precursor of modern sorting tests for the assessment of executive deficits[15]. Goldstein’s concept of the abstract attitude as a ‘marker’ of brain damage has been criticized as vague and unreliable for neuropsychological
assessment[16]. Nevertheless, it remains a groundbreaking inspiration not only for neuropsychologists,
but also philosophers[17]
-
[19]. In any case, Goldstein and Scheerer’s[3] urge for qualitative assessments in neuropsychology is at least as important today
as it was 80 years ago.