Keywords:
Dementia - cognition - cognition disorders - memory
Palavras-chave:
Demência - cognição - transtornos cognitivos - memória
Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a condition that lies between the normal aging-associated
cognitive decline and dementia. It involves a degree of cognitive decline that is
not sufficient to impair an individual's daily living activities. It is estimated
that the prevalence of MCI is 1020% in individuals aged 65 years or older and that
it increases with age[1].
Within this context, various cognitive tests are available for the screening and diagnosis
of MCI and early-stage dementia. The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) is the most
commonly used test[2]. The MMSE has a sensitivity of 62.7% and a specificity of 63.3% when used to detect
MCI in healthy individuals[3]. It is important, however, to take into account age, gender and education in the
interpretation of MMSE scores, especially in populations with lower educational levels[4],[5].
Another less commonly used instrument is the Brief Cognitive Screening Battery (BCSB).
The BCSB consists of the following tests: identification and naming of 10 common objects
from drawings (sensitivity: 50%; specificity: 96.66%); incidental recall (sensitivity:
90%; specificity: 83.33%), immediate recall and learning of these images; the clock-drawing
test (sensitivity: 90%; specificity: 83.33%); semantic verbal fluency test based on
naming animals (sensitivity: 96.66%; specificity: 93.33%); delayed recall (sensitivity:
93.33%; specificity: 96.66%) and recognition of the drawings used in the first test
when presented among other drawings (sensitivity: 93.33%; specificity: 90%)[6],[7].
Scores on the MMSE, the verbal fluency test and the clock-drawing test differ between
literate and illiterate individuals without dementia. On the other hand, this difference
is not observed on the BCSB delayed recall test scores[8]. The BCSB is a highly accurate method for the diagnosis of Alzheimer's disease in
both illiterate and literate individuals[9].
We hypothesized that education influences the performance of individuals more significantly
on the MMSE than on the BCSB.
For the purpose of testing this hypothesis, the major objective of this study was
to compare the influence of educational levels on the MMSE and BCSB scores.
METHODS
This was a cross-sectional observational study of 112 healthy individuals aged between
60 and 80 years, who attended outpatient clinics of the Hospital de Clínicas of the
Federal University of Parana, located in Curitiba, Brazil. Participants were randomized
by a simple randomization technique. Ten patients were chosen per day from a list
of patients who attended the outpatient clinics that day. These selected patients
were addressed, then were included or not in the study according to the inclusion
and exclusion criteria and their acceptance of the consent terms. All the individuals
performed both tests and each test was conducted by a different evaluator blind to
the other test's results (the MMSE was always conducted by evaluator 1 and the BCSB
was always conducted by evaluator 2). We chose this design to minimize measurement
bias. Patients attending neurology and psychiatry outpatient clinics were excluded,
as were those using barbiturates, benzodiazepines, anticholinesterases, antipsychotics
and thyroid hormones, because these criteria could predict the presence of major cognitive
declines or neurologic or psychiatric comorbidities that could impact cognitive functioning[10]. Therefore, these patients were excluded, to minimize the risk of including cognitively
impaired individuals. However, we did not perform any diagnostic test for the exclusions,
because we aimed to mimic the reality of a primary care context in which accurate
diagnostic testing might not be available. The study was approved by the Human Research
Ethics Committee of the Hospital de Clínicas of the Federal University of Parana.
Participants were categorized on the basis of their education (group 0: illiterate;
group 1: 1–4 years of education; group 2: 5-8 years of education; group 3: more than
eight years of education). The MMSE was administered to all participants according
to the procedure described by Brucki et al.[11] and the BCSB was assessed as described by Vitiello et al.[6].
Scores for the various components of the BCSB were calculated separately and only
the scores for the delayed recall test, clock-drawing test and verbal fluency test
were used in the analysis. These tests were chosen from the BCSB because they evaluate
important cognitive domains impaired in MCI and dementia, such as memory in the delayed
recall test, executive function and language in the verbal fluency test, and visuospatial
skills in the clock-drawing test. They were chosen instead of the other tests because
they had the highest sensitivity and specificity in the domain they evaluate. If we
included all the tests in the analysis, the results would have been redundant and
less reliable[12],[13]. However, the time taken to apply the whole BCSB was used in the application time
analysis.
For the analysis we used cut-off points proposed for the Brazilian population. The
cut-off points for the MMSE were as follows: 20 for group 0; 25 for group 1; 26 for
group 2; 28 for group 3[11]. The cut-off points for the BCSB tests analyzed in our study were: delayed recall
test: 6; verbal fluency test: 13[7]. We did not analyze performance on the clock-drawing test based on cut-off points
because participants who refused to perform that test were excluded from its analysis.
Statistical analysis was carried out using R Statistical Software, 2016. Test administration
times were compared using the Mann-Whitney test. The effects of educational level
were assessed using the Kruskal-Wallis test and pairwise multiple comparisons.
RESULTS
[Table 1] shows the educational profile of the sample. Eighty participants (71%) were women.
Median assessment times for the MMSE and BCSB were respectively 5 and 8 minutes (p
< 0.0001).
Table 1
Distribution of participants by level of education.
Groups
|
Level of education
|
Total
|
Relative (%)
|
Group 0
|
10
|
9
|
Group 1
|
44
|
39
|
Group 2
|
29
|
26
|
Group 3
|
29
|
26
|
Total
|
112
|
100
|
[Table 2] presents a group comparison of median scores on the MMSE, delayed test, clock-drawing
test and verbal fluency test. Regarding the MMSE scores, all the pairwise comparisons
between groups were significant (p < 0.0001), except for the comparison between groups
1 and 2 (p = 0.1189).
Table 2
Median scores for the MMSE and for the BCSB selected tests for analysis (delayed recall
test, clock-drawing test and verbal fluency test).
Test
|
Median score
|
Group 0
|
Group 1
|
Group 2
|
Group 3
|
p-value
|
MMSE
|
21
|
23
|
23
|
26
|
< 0.0001
|
Delayed recall test
|
7
|
8
|
7
|
8
|
0.0804
|
Clock-drawing test
|
4
|
8
|
9
|
9
|
< 0.0001
|
Verbal fluency test
|
12
|
14
|
12
|
18
|
0.00035
|
MMSE: Mini-mental state examination; BCSB: Brief cognitive battery.
Similarly, pairwise comparisons of scores on the clock-drawing test were also significant
(p < 0.0001) except for the comparison of groups 1 and 2 (p = 0.082). Fifteen participants
(13%) refused to draw the clock and were therefore excluded from this analysis. The
distribution of refusals by education group was as follows, group 0: n = 6 (60%);
group 1: n = 6 (14%); group 2: n = 2 (7%); group 3: n = 1 (3%).
Pairwise comparison revealed that the performance of group 3 on the verbal fluency
test was different from that of all the other groups: vs. group 0: p = 0.0032; vs.
group 1: p = 0.0003; vs. group 2: p = 0.0001. All the other groups had similar verbal
fluency test scores: group 0 vs. group 1: p = 0.5596; group 0 vs. group 2: p = 0.9618;
group 1 vs. group 2: p = 0.435.
Delayed recall test scores were similar in all groups (p = 0.0804).
DISCUSSION
The median MMSE scores in our sample are comparable to those reported by Brucki et
al.[11]: illiterates, MMSE = 20; 1–4 years of education, MMSE = 25; 5–8 years of education,
MMSE = 26.5; over eight years of education, MMSE = 28. However, there is some debate
about which cut-off points should be used for the MMSE. Brucki et al.[11] suggested that the cut-off points should be analyzed for each individual separately,
whereas Bertolucci et al.[14] suggested values of 13 (illiterates), 18 (1–8 years of education) and 26 (more than
eight years of education), and Almeida et al.[15] suggested 20 as a cut-off for illiterate individuals. In a study that compared illiterate
and literate individuals, the median MMSE scores were 21 and 26.5 for illiterate and
literate participants respectively[8]. In summary, the analysis of MMSE scores has substantially changed over time.
Education significantly influences MMSE scores and has been reported to be the most
important determinant of variance in performance on the test[13],[16]. Therefore, most of the Brazilian studies on the MMSE performed between 1998 and
2013 used educational criteria to define cut-off points[17]. Our results also suggest that education is a decisive element in MMSE performance.
Thus, the lack of standardization for the MMSE, shown by the significant variability
of the scores among different educational levels, impairs its validity[17].
As discussed previously, the weaknesses of the MMSE have gradually become more apparent.
Therefore, the urge to provide an assessment of cognition better than the MMSE, which
is valid for the entire Brazilian population, became even more evident. The BCSB has
become the more appropriate option for that purpose. The tests that make up the BCSB
have been studied since 1994. In fact, the accuracy of the selected tests for the
BCSB is high, and comparable to that of the MMSE[7].
Our study suggests that education more significantly influences the MMSE than the
BCSB in our population. Our sample showed that the delayed recall test scores did
not vary with levels of education. Nitrini et al.[7] reported that illiterate and literate participants, and participants with little
education and standard education, obtained similar scores on the BCSB delayed recall
test. They concluded that, unlike the MMSE, the delayed recall test is not educationally
biased[8].
The effect of educational level on the verbal fluency test that we observed was due
to the performance of the group with more than eight years of education. This is consistent
with another study, which reported that individuals with more than eight years of
education spoke approximately six more words than individuals with less education[18]. Another study also found a significant difference between the verbal fluency scores
of illiterate and literate participants[8]. In our sample, participants with more than eight years of schooling spoke between
four and six more words than participants in the other groups.
In our sample, educational level influenced performance on the clock-drawing test.
Previous studies have also reported that individuals with more education perform better
than those with less education[19],[20]. It should be noted, however, that our analysis of the clock-drawing test was impaired
by the high rate of refusals to perform the test, especially in the illiterate group.
In most cases the refusal was due to lack of knowledge and embarrassment about having
their drawings evaluated. This indicates a limitation for the clinical use of the
clock-drawing test in individuals with less education.
We found that the MMSE took three minutes less to assess than the BCSB. The median
assessment time for the BCSB in our sample, eight minutes, is similar to the median
times reported in other studies[9]. The three-minute difference in application time is not a barrier to the clinical
use of the BCSB.
While the BCSB takes longer to apply, tests that minimize educational bias, such as
the delayed recall test of the BCSB, are more appropriate to the Brazilian context.
The heterogeneity of the Brazilian population makes it difficult to standardize scores
on tests that are influenced by the educational level.
Besides the fact that our sample was selected from outpatient clinics from a tertiary
health service, the proportion of each education group was compatible to the profile
of users of the Brazilian public health system, as stated by Ribeiro et al.[21]: up to three years of education (groups 0 and 1): 41.9%; four to seven years (group
2): 24%; over eight years (group 3): 34.1%. The small number of illiterates in our
sample made the observations in this group less reliable, but that number can be justified
by the percentage of illiterate people in the Brazilian population, which was 8% in
2015 according to data from the Brazilian National Institute of Geography and Statistics[22]. Therefore, our sample possibly reflects the reality of the Brazilian population.
The main weaknesses of our study are the small number of illiterates and the sample
selected from a tertiary hospital. As stated before, both weaknesses could impair
external validity but the proportions of participants in each group were consistent
with some data already published. Therefore, we suggest, although we cannot confirm,
that our results could be representative of the Brazilian population. Also, we did
not carry out any additional testing for illiterates who refused to perform the clock-drawing
test, which significantly impaired that analysis.
Further studies are necessary to assess the need for new cut-off points for the clock-drawing
and verbal fluency tests and to develop more appropriate tools to assess visuospatial
functions in illiterate individuals.