Keywords social skills interventions - social communication - social cognition - autism spectrum
disorder - performance-and theater-based social skills interventions
Learning Outcomes: As a result of this activity, the reader will be able to (1) describe social skills
interventions and contrast between knowledge-based and performance-based social skills
interventions; (2) consider several performance- and theater-based social skills interventions
with empirical support; (3) evaluate social cognition and social communication outcomes
for autistic individuals following performance- and theater-based social skills interventions.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD)[a ] is characterized by differences in social communication, such as altered social–emotional
reciprocity and difficulties in developing peer relationships, and restricted interests
and/or repetitive behaviors, which may include vocal and/or motor stereotypy.[1 ] Recent prevalence estimates suggest that ASD is diagnosed in 1 in 44 children[2 ]; those diagnosed with ASD may manifest a range of speech and language difficulties,
such as expressive and receptive language deficits and impairment in pragmatic language.[3 ]
[4 ]
Social communication and social interaction difficulties involving reciprocal communication,
using nonverbal communication, and developing peer relationships are commonly seen
in youth with ASD, and are closely related to differences in social cognition. Social
cognition broadly includes affect recognition, theory of mind (ToM), and formal social
knowledge (e.g., social norms; knowledge of what the appropriate social behavior is
in a given social situation).[5 ]
[6 ]
[7 ]
[8 ] Affect or emotion recognition is the ability to identify emotional states based
on visual and auditory nonverbal cues.[9 ] Deficits in affect recognition may predict ToM deficits in autistic individuals.[10 ] ToM is the ability to infer, comprehend, and reason about the mental and affective
states of self (intrapersonal ToM) or others (interpersonal ToM); this is also referred
to as perspective-taking.[11 ]
[12 ]
[13 ]
[14 ] Individuals with ASD have ToM impairments compared with age-matched typically-developing
(TD) peers that have been attributed to differences in social information processing.[5 ] Social information processing is an essential component of social cognition which
encompasses a combination of important skills like social problem solving and comprehension
of implicit and explicit verbal and social cues, and impacts acquisition of social
knowledge.[15 ] Although TD individuals develop these skills from sociocultural learning experiences
in daily life, autistic individuals often need support to acquire these skills and
apply them in social settings.[16 ] Without the development of these skills, individuals with ASD may have difficulties
forming friendships, understanding social environments, and navigating real-world
social situations. Furthermore, due to these differences, autistic individuals are
at risk for peer rejection and social isolation, which may contribute to lasting mental
health difficulties in this population.[17 ] Therefore, it is important to address these differences in social development to
mitigate risk and improve long-term outcomes.[18 ]
[19 ]
Social skills interventions (SSIs) are among the most commonly-used intervention approaches
for improving social functioning in youth with ASD.[20 ] SSIs provide support around social communication skills and seek to improve social
cognition.[21 ] Overall, both individual and group-based SSIs have shown promise for improving social
skills in youth with ASD as summarized in several review articles.[18 ]
[19 ]
[20 ]
[22 ] A recent meta-analysis by Gates and colleagues[18 ] examined the efficacy (i.e., the examination of a treatment under a controlled circumstance, randomized
controlled trials [RCTs]) of SSIs in group format, indicating an overall medium effect.
Many group-based SSIs also demonstrate effectiveness (i.e., examination of the intervention's outcomes in a community or uncontrolled
setting) for improving social skills, with some secondary effects such as improved
psychological well-being.[23 ] However, the effects appear to differ by informant: while effects were largest for
self-report, they were attributable entirely to improved social knowledge, not self-reported
change in social behavior. These findings pose questions as to whether different approaches
in teaching social skills may affect changes in social knowledge versus social behaviors.[18 ]
SSIs can take different forms depending on the specific target skills and instructional
methodologies used. The two most popular approaches are (1) structured learning or
knowledge-based SSIs and (2) performance-based SSIs (PBSSIs). The former approach
is more traditional and focuses on didactic, explicit teaching in individual or group
formats. These SSIs resemble an instructional setting (e.g., classroom) where topics
covering multiple domains of social functioning are reviewed, followed by a practice
and a feedback component with a clinician (e.g., role-play, rehearsal).[24 ]
[25 ] By contrast, PBSSIs are typically conducted in a group format where social skills
are implicitly learned through spontaneous peer interactions by engaging in fun activities
together in a supported environment, rather than through didactic instruction.[26 ] One of the defining features of PBSSIs is the emphasis on tapping into the intrinsic
motivation of the participants during peer interactions, hence making the social interaction
more motivating and reinforcing.[9 ] Although social knowledge-based SSIs teach specific target skills such as appropriate
behaviors in social contexts (i.e., social knowledge training) with predetermined
structured activities, PBSSIs incorporate interest-based learning without needing
to modify the content of the intervention, resulting in a more flexible approach.
Moreover, children with ASD may have special interests (e.g., cartoon characters,
tangible items) which could be intrinsically motivating for them that can easily be
integrated as part of PBSSIs. This, in turn, may lead to increased treatment efficacy
and effectiveness.[9 ] In short, PBSSIs aim to provide an enriched environment that successfully promotes
and reinforces naturalistic peer interactions.
While PBSSIs vary in terms of activities or strategies used (e.g., play-based activities,
games, sports, music, and/or dance), one set of PBSSI strategies that has gained more
attention with a growing body of literature is drama- or theater-based approaches .[20 ]
[26 ]
[27 ]
[28 ]
[29 ]
[30 ] Training in acting techniques has many parallels with the skills targeted in SSIs:
theater/acting activities naturally provide an opportunity for social interaction.
Furthermore, theater and acting activities involve perceiving and interpreting social
information from others and responding to it, as well as paying attention to how oneself
expresses ideas. Additionally, acting games and improvisation can theoretically promote
imagination and cognitive flexibility.[28 ]
[31 ] Notwithstanding the efficacy and effectiveness of these theater-based SSIs (TBSSIs)
as reported in the extant literature, no review to date specifically focuses on synthesizing
social cognition and communication outcomes of empirically validated TBSSIs. Therefore,
the aims of this review article are to (1) identify and describe TBSSIs that have
been evaluated in the extant literature and (2) characterize the evidence of TBSSIs
for supporting specific aspects of social cognition and social communication in individuals
with ASD.
Method
Identification of Studies
An examination of extant literature was performed to identify relevant studies for
this review. PsycInfo, PsycArticles, and PubMed databases were searched from the start
of the database until August 2021. The search was conducted using terms referencing
ASD (i.e., autism, autism spectrum disorder, Asperger, pervasive developmental disorder),
social skills and related constructs of interest (i.e., social skills, social cognition,
social communication, peer relationships, social competence), and key types of treatments
(i.e., performance-based, theater-based interventions). The following Boolean string
was utilized: (ASD OR autism spectrum disorder OR Asperger OR autis * OR pervasive developmental disorder) AND (social skills OR social cognition OR social
communication OR social OR peer relationships OR social competence) AND (theater based
OR performance based OR improv OR theater-based treatment OR theater-based intervention
OR theater-based therapy OR performance-based treatment OR performance-based intervention
OR performance-based therapy ). Data management was conducted using the Rayyan reference manager software and Microsoft
Excel.
Study Selection and Literature Search
The following inclusion criteria were used in the title and abstract and full-text
review process: (1) articles were original empirical research in a peer-reviewed journal;
(2) written in the English language; (3) included a sample of participants meeting
DSM-V or DSM-IV criteria for ASD diagnosed by either a clinician or a standardized
measure for ASD (including ASD, autism, Asperger's disorder, pervasive developmental
disorder—not otherwise specified); (4) included a TBSSI, defined by the study team
and previous literature as a targeted SSI that provided implicit learning of social
skills in an unstructured setting, specifically using drama, improvisation, and other
theater-based techniques[32 ]
[33 ]; (5) included quantitative data specifically on social communication (e.g., reciprocal
communication) or social cognition (e.g., ToM), as measured by behavioral tasks and
parent-, self-, or observer-report questionnaires; and (6) was not music therapy,
dance therapy, or early intervention: these categories of interventions were excluded
due to the differing goals of the treatments (e.g., to improve general emotional well-being)
and the lack of explicit focus on improving social skills competence, the significant
variability between interventions, and the lack of specific theater- and drama-based
social skills components (i.e., theater elements, improvisation, development of social
play). After removal of duplicate articles, a team of three independent reviewers
conducted title and abstract review, with excellent interrater agreement (97.2%) on
decisions on inclusion of articles on 20% of screened articles. The full-text review
was performed independently by four authors to ensure accurate inclusion. Any disagreements
between reviewers were discussed and resolved by group consensus and through consultation
with the last author. Finally, a backward search of the reference list for the included
articles and a forward search were conducted to ensure all available articles were
included in the review.
Results
Overview of Identified Studies
The search yielded 963 articles, of which 59 were included for full-text review. Eleven
articles met criteria and were included in this review ([Table 1 ]). The selection process is reported in the flow diagram ([Fig. 1 ]).
Table 1
Summary of Included Studies of Performance- and Theater-Based Social Skills Intervention
Author
N
Age and IQ
Intervention name and study design
Social communication outcome measure
Social cognition outcome measure
Results
Authors' conclusions
Corbett et al[33 ]
N = 8
(7 males)
Age
ASD: M = 11.30, SD = 3.98
Range: 6–17 y
IQ
M = 82.36, SD = 16.44
SENSE Theatre
Pre-post uncontrolled
SRS; SSP; Adaptive Behavior Assessment System
NEPSY-II: AR, MF, ToM
Improvements in memory for faces (d = 1.68) and ToM skills (d = 1.44). No significant results for affect recognition (d = 0.62) or parent-reported measures of sensory sensitivity, adaptive behaviors, and
autism severity
SENSE Theatre has the potential to improve social communication and social cognition
differences in youth with ASD
Corbett et al[39 ]
N = 12
(9 males)
Age
M = 12.17
Range: 8–17 years
IQ
M = 82 (74 -118)
SENSE Theatre
Pre-post uncontrolled
SRS; SCQ; Adaptive Behavior Assessment System; companionship scale; PIP
NEPSY-II: AR and MF (immediate and delayed)
Participants displayed improvements in peer engagement (companionship scale), social
awareness (SRS; d = 0.23), social cognition (SRS; d = 1.46), memory for faces delayed (NEPSY-II; d = 0.89), parent-reported home living (ABAS; d = 0.34), and self-care (ABAS; d = 0.29). No effect for home-based communication and social domains (ABAS; d = 0.28, 0.09, respectively), amount of eye contact, affect recognition (NEPSY-II;
d = 0.18), immediate memory for faces (NEPSY-II; d = 0.51), or cooperative play (PIP)
SENSE Theatre increased peer interaction and was associated with improvements in social
awareness, social cognition, and facial processing. SENSE Theatre has the potential
to reduce core social differences related to ASD
Corbett et al[28 ]
N = 30
(TG = 17; WLC = 13)
Age
SENSE:
M = 11.27, SD = 2.51
WLC: M = 10.74, SD = 1.89
Range:
8–14 y
IQ
>70
SENSE Theatre
Pre-post follow-up WLC
SRS; PIP; ABAS
NEPSY-II: ToM and MF (immediate and delayed)
Significant improvements in social communication at post-test (SRS; d = 0.86) and follow-up (d = 0.82). Improvement in daily social functioning (ABAS) at post-test (d = 0.77; not maintained at follow-up), and group play (d = 0.77). Significant improvements in contextual ToM (d = 0.99) but not verbal ToM (NEPSY-II). Improvement in immediate memory for faces
(d = 0.75; marginal), delayed memory for faces (d = 0.98), and ERP measure of incidental memory for faces (d = 0.93)
SENSE Theatre improves social communication and social interaction, facial memory,
and theory of mind
Corbett et al[29 ]
N = 77
(TG = 44; WLC = 33)
Age
SENSE:
M = 11.12, SD = 2.54
WLC:
M = 10.58, SD = 2.32
Range: 8–14 y
IQ
>70
SENSE Theatre
Pre-post WLC
PIP
NEPSY-II: ToM; incidental face paradigm
Significant group differences were noted during solicited play (d = 0.58), but not unsolicited play (d = 0.48). There were increased verbal interactions during solicited play (d = 0.47; cooperative play [PIP]). Significant group differences were also noted in
ERP markers of incidental face memory and verbal ToM (NEPSY-II; d = 0.45), but not in contextual ToM (NEPSY-II; d = 0.38)
SENSE Theatre improves several aspects of social cognition, such as theory of mind,
and social communication in youth with ASD
Ioannou et al[40 ]
N = 77
(59 males; all ASD)
(TG = 44; WLC = 33)
Age
Range: 10–17 y
IQ
>70
SENSE Theatre
Pre-post WLC
PIP
–
Improvement in solicited group play and individual play (i.e., individual play decreased).
No differences in unsolicited group or solitary play (PIP). Significant reductions
in trait (but not state anxiety) in the treatment group at post-test following group
play
SENSE Theatre contributes to an improvement in social abilities and a reduction in
trait anxiety for youth with ASD
Lerner et al[37 ]
N = 17
(14 males)
TG = 9
(8 males)
Control = 8 (6 males)
Age
SDARI:
M = 14.31, SD = 1.328;
Control:
M = 14.32, SD = 1.931
Range: 10–17 y
IQ
N/A
SDARI
Pre-post follow-up nonrandomized control
SSRS; SRS; EDI; Child Behavior Checklist
DANVA-2
Increased social assertion and emotion recognition in adult voices. Reduced social
problems at follow-up. No effect for parent-reported overall social skills, nonverbal
communication, parent- or self-reported internalizing and externalizing problems
SDARI is effective for addressing areas of social skills development for youth with
ASD, and some effects seem to be sustained during the follow-up period
Lerner and Mikami[7 ]
N = 13
(all male)
SDARI: n = 7
Skillstreaming: n = 6
Age
SDARI:
M = 10.86, SD = 1.68;
Skillstreaming: M = 11.33, SD = 1.63
IQ
N/A
SDARI
Pre-post
RCT
SCQ; SRS; SIOS; participant sociometric nominations
Relative decrease in social preference (η2 = 0.70) than Skillstreaming. More general interaction in SDARI participants at Session
1 but decrease over time (d = 1.80) compared with Skillstreaming. Significant improvements in reciprocal friendships
(η2 = 0.31) and social skills (η2 = 0.59) for both groups over time, but no between-group differences over time in
these domains.
No effect for parent-reported social behavior
Due to its reinforcing nature, SDARI promotes rapid peer liking and social interaction,
with very slight decreases in these domains over time
Marro et al[26 ]
N = 69
(49 males)
Study 1 : n = 56
(40 males)
Study 2 : n = 13
(9 males)
Age
Study 1:
M = 12.376, SD = 2.90
Study 2:
M = 13.25, SD = 2.14
IQ
>70
SDARI
Study 1: RCT
Study 2 : pre-post uncontrolled
–
CABS; TASSK; ToMI-2; DANVA-2
Study 1: Improved social cognition and ToM at post-test, but not emotion recognition.
Study 2 : Improved ToM and emotion recognition post-test, but not social cognition
SDARI improves social cognition, ToM, and FER. Specific gains depend on the setting
in which the intervention is conducted
Beadle-Brown et al[30 ]
N = 22
(18 males)
Age
M = 9.16
Range: 7–12 y
IQ
Range: 29–87
Imagining Autism
Pre-post follow-up
Uncontrolled
ADOS-2; VABS; researcher-created parent and teacher Likert-type measure to assess
ASD impact; parent and teacher interviews
FER: modified version of the Ekman (1993) faces task
Reduced autism severity as per parent and teacher interviews and per ADOS-2, improved
reciprocal social interaction (d = 1.96), and improvements in parent-reported socialization (VABS; d = 3.42) and communication (VABS; d = 6.07). Emotion recognition improved from baseline to follow-up (d = 2.12), but not from pre- to post-test
Imagining Autism is a feasible and enjoyable intervention for children with ASD
Mehling et al[45 ]
N = 14
Age
M = 12.42
IQ
N/A
The Heartbeat Method
Pre-post
Uncontrolled
ADOS-2; VABS; Test of Pragmatic Language-2; Social Validity Questionnaire
Penn Facial Recognition Task
Improvements in socialization (VABS), expressive language (VABS), and relationships
(VABS) and improvement in pragmatic language.
No effect for FER for the whole sample, but subgroup with lower scores at baseline
did improve (n = 7)
The Heartbeat Method is effective for improving domains of socialization and communication,
as well as improving pragmatic language
Guli et al[27 ]
N = 34
(28 males);
ASD = 18; NLD = 8; ADHD = 8
Age
ASD:
M = 10.97
Range: 8–14 y
IQ
>80
SCIP
Pre-Post WLC
SSRS; Behavior Assessment System for Children; observed social interaction (positive
interactions and solitary behaviors); social validity semistructured interviews (parent
and child)
DANVA-2
(child faces and child voices)
Improvement in positive interactions and decrease in solitary play.
Parent- and child-reported improvements in social cognition and social communication.
No effect for social and behavioral adjustment or nonverbal cue reading
SCIP can effectively address difficulties with social competence in children with
ASD and related disorders
Note. ABAS, Adaptive Behavior Assessment System; ADHD, Attention-deficit/hyperactivity
disorder; ADOS-2, Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule- 2; AR, Affect Recognition;
ASD, autism spectrum disorder; BASC, Behavioral Assessment System for Children; CABS,
Children's Assertiveness Behavior Scale; CBCL, Child Behavior Checklist; DANVA-2,
Diagnostic analysis of nonverbal accuracy- 2; EDI, Emory Dyssemia Index; ERP, Event
Related Potential; FACT, Fun Activities for Children and Teens; FER, facial emotion
recognition; MF, Memory for Faces; NEPSY-II, A Developmental NEuroPSYchological Assessment;
NLD, Nonverbal Learning Disability; PIP, Peer Interaction paradigm; RCT, Randomized
Controlled Trial; SCQ, Social Communication Questionnaire; SCIP, Social Competence
Intervention Program; SDARI, Socio-dramatic affective-relational intervention; SIOS,
Social Interaction Observation System; SSP, Short Sensory Profile; SSRS, Social Skills
Rating System; SRS, Social Responsiveness Scale; TASSK, Test of Adolescent Social
Skills Knowledge; TG, Treatment Group; ToM, Theory of Mind; ToMI, Theory of Mind Inventory;
VABS, Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scale; WLC, Wait list control.
d = Cohen's d , an effect size measure that indicates the standardized differences between means
such that 0.2 constitutes a small effect while 0.5 corresponds to a medium effect
size, and 0.8 indicates a large effect size.[72 ]
η2 = eta squared, an effect size measure that represents the proportion of variance
in the dependent variable that is accounted for by the independent variable. For this
metric, 0.01 indicates a small effect, 0.06 indicates a medium effect, and 0.14 indicates
a large effect.[72 ]
Figure 1 Identification and selection of included studies.
Five named (i.e., identified by the authors as denoting a specific approach) TBSSIs
were identified from the search: Social Emotional NeuroScience Endocrinology (SENSE)
Theatre, Socio-dramatic Affective-relational Intervention (SDARI), Imagining Autism,
the Hunter Heartbeat Method, and the Social Competence Intervention Program (SCIP).
All identified interventions included well-characterized autistic samples, with 7
out of 11 studies using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition
(ADOS-2),[34 ] a gold-standard autism diagnostic tool, to confirm ASD diagnosis.[35 ] Four articles utilized a record review of clinician diagnosis to confirm diagnosis
of ASD.[16 ]
[27 ]
[36 ]
[37 ] All identified interventions are intended for individuals with autism between the
ages of 6 and 17 years. The characteristics and findings from the identified studies
are described later.
Social Emotional NeuroScience Endocrinology Theatre
Description. SENSE Theatre is a 2- to 12-week, peer-mediated theater-based intervention developed
specifically for youth with ASD that has taken place in various settings, such as
universities, school auditoriums, and summer camps.[38 ] This TBSSI utilizes social interaction with peers through theater acting, to target
core differences associated with ASD, such as ToM, social communication, and flexible
and imaginative thinking.[28 ] The process of acting involves many essential aspects of social communication such
as socializing, perceiving emotion, and thought expression, which allows participants
to learn these skills through theatric approaches.[28 ] The 10 core objectives of SENSE Theatre intervention are to (1) provide social support
in the form of building trust; (2) create an environment that is enjoyable and that
involves social play; (3) replicate warm social interaction with peers, especially
reciprocal interactions; (4) enhance motivation (increase social initiation); (5)
engage in directed, reciprocal communication; (6) utilize nonverbal communication
skills (improving eye contact, facial expressions, and gestures); (7) engage in imaginative
play; (8) respond to others empathically; (9) support active learning by introducing
novelty and encouraging participation; and (10) advance individual learning by using
social knowledge to inform social behavior.[39 ] SENSE Theatre seeks to address differences that are characteristic of ASD utilizing
specific techniques such as role-playing, improvisation, and character development.[39 ] Initial sessions include theater games and imaginative play, while subsequent sessions
include encouraging the participant to think about the development of a character.
The intervention culminates in a public performance of the play.[28 ]
[29 ]
[39 ] Notably, a unique aspect of this intervention is pairing individuals with ASD with
a TD peer that has received training about ASD, the intervention, and approaches for
addressing behavioral challenges they may experience when working with autistic peers.
These trained TD peers provide real-time, naturalistic models of reciprocal social
communication skills to facilitate generalization of learned skills.[28 ]
Current empirical evidence. To date, five studies have been conducted examining the effects of SENSE Theatre
across a range of development periods, with each displaying some specific and general
improvements in social cognition and communication.[28 ]
[29 ]
[33 ]
[39 ]
[40 ]
The first study examining SENSE Theatre was an uncontrolled pilot study, conducted
with participants aged 6 to 17 years with ASD across a period of 3 months. Participants
completed between one and four sessions, for approximately 2 hours per week depending
on their role in the play, which was performed for the public. Participants showed
improvements in face identification and memory (i.e., a measure for identifying familiar
vs. novel faces following a delay), as well as some improvement in ToM skills from
pre- to post-intervention ([Table 1 ]). No improvements were observed from pre- to post-treatment in affect recognition,
parent-reported adaptive behaviors, or ratings of autism severity.[33 ]
A subsequent uncontrolled study (N = 12; 8–17 years old) was conducted during a 2-week (4 hours per day; 5 days/week)
summer camp followed by two public performances. Results from this study indicated
increases in overall ASD symptoms, specifically in social awareness and social cognition,
and active engagement with peers as well as specific increases in delayed face identification memory ([Table 1 ]). However, there were no effects of treatment on eye contact, affect recognition,
immediate facial identification memory (i.e., recognizing a previously seen faces following
a brief 5-second initial exposure), or parent-reported adaptive functioning in the
domains of communication and social functioning.[39 ]
Two RCTs have been conducted to examine the efficacy of SENSE Theatre in a summer
camp setting (4-hour weekly sessions for 10 weeks, followed by two public plays).[28 ]
[29 ]
[40 ] The first RCT was conducted with 30 autistic children (treatment: n = 17; waitlist control [WLC]: n = 13) aged 8 to 14 years. Participants displayed improvements in parent-reported
ASD-related social communication skills as well as daily social functioning. Notably,
changes in social communication were maintained at 2-month follow-up, but changes
in social functioning were not maintained. When assessing social interaction variables,
improvements were found in the children's play with peers. Participants also improved
in several social cognition domains, including contextual ToM, which refers to one's ability to relate emotionally to a social context, demonstrating
the skills to comprehend another individual's emotional experience.[28 ] Additionally, participants in the treatment condition displayed improvements in
both immediate (marginal) and delayed facial memory.[28 ]
In a subsequent RCT, SENSE Theatre was conducted with 77 autistic children and adolescents
8 to 16 years of age (treatment: n = 44; WLC: n = 33). Participants in the treatment group evinced improved performance in verbal ToM, which refers to the understanding that another person's thoughts and feelings
may be independent from one's own (e.g., false-belief tasks, understanding figurative
language). However, no improvements in contextual ToM were noted. Increased neural evidence of memory for face as indexed by electroencephalogram
(EEG) was displayed in the treatment group.[29 ]
Additional findings from this RCT study demonstrated that children in the treatment
group exhibited increased engagement with peers and less individual play when invited
by a confederate (solicited play). There were no differences between groups in unsolicited
play (initiated by the participant, but not invited by a confederate).[29 ]
[40 ] Additionally, the treatment group showed greater improvement in trait but not state
anxiety following a group play with new children.[40 ] As social interaction may be a source of increased stress for individuals with ASD,
it is notable that this intervention has been shown to reduce the level of stress
and anxiety in individuals with ASD.
Overall, SENSE Theatre appears to be both efficacious and effective for treating particular
aspects of social communication and social cognition in children with ASD. These include
improvements in ASD-related social communication, social awareness, more frequent
peer interactions, and delayed face identification and memory, which was corroborated by changes in neural index
of face memory.[28 ]
[29 ]
[39 ]
[40 ] Findings were mixed across studies in terms of results related to parent-reported
adaptive functioning (including social functioning and communication), different types
of ToM (e.g., contextual vs. verbal ), and immediate facial memory. However, there were other aspects where no change in symptoms was
evinced, including eye contact,[39 ] unsolicited group and self-play,[29 ]
[40 ] and affect recognition.[33 ]
[39 ]
Socio-Dramatic Affective-Relational Intervention
Description. SDARI is a TBSSI that utilizes specialized games and age-appropriate motivators to
foster social motivation and creativity to provide positive social reinforcement and
relationship-building opportunities in children and adolescents with ASD.[31 ]
[37 ] The SDARI approach is highly adaptable and uses activities that intrinsically motivate
participants so as to create situations that more closely parallel authentic interactions
in social contexts. SDARI's games target skills including perspective-taking, nonverbal
communication, interpretation of others' body language, and cooperation, and is selected
based on age and interest of group members to maximize engagement.[31 ] For example, in a game called “gibberish,” one participant speaks in nonsensical
sounds while “describing” how to perform a common task. Another participant must watch
and then translate the gibberish into words for the other participants.[37 ] In this game, participants are encouraged to interpret the subtle nonverbal information
being conveyed by the first participant to translate the message. In this way, the
SDARI approach more closely parallels authentic interactions in social contexts.
SDARI can be delivered in both laboratory-based, controlled environments and community-based
settings, and has been flexibly implemented with high fidelity (i.e., while maintaining
adherence to the manualized approach and conceptual principles of the intervention)
across a wide range of formats, from 4 sessions (90 minutes)[7 ] to 10 sessions (also 90 minutes each), as well as a 6-week summer camp (e.g., 5-hour,
daily sessions).[26 ]
[37 ]
Current empirical evidence. Three studies have been conducted to explore the effectiveness of the SDARI protocol
in areas related to social cognition and communication differences.[7 ]
[26 ]
[37 ]
The first study of SDARI was a controlled pilot study that assessed 17 participants
(treatment: n = 9; no-intervention control: n = 8) aged 11 to 17 years old, where the treatment group participated in a 6-week
summer camp version of SDARI.[37 ] Effects of treatment were examined at post-intervention and at a 6-week follow-up.
Post-intervention improvements were reflected in increases in parents' reports of
their children's assertion and improvements in the performance on the vocal emotion
identification task, which were maintained at follow-up. Although not significant
at post-treatment, parent-reported social problems (i.e., peer rejection and teasing)
were significantly improved at follow-up, suggesting a delayed effect of treatment.
Social cognition outcomes measured using a vocal emotion recognition task also improved
at post-treatment and follow-up. No effect for other measures of social skills or
ASD-like nonverbal communication skills was observed.
In a subsequent RCT,[7 ] 13 participants were randomly assigned to either SDARI (n = 7) or Skillstreaming (n = 6), which is a didactic, knowledge-based SSI. The duration of the intervention was
one 90-minute session per week for 4 weeks. Participants' social interactions, as
assessed by behavioral coding, reflected decreased positive and negative interactions
in SDARI participants compared with Skillstreaming,[41 ] while on the sociometric outcomes, both groups increased in reciprocated friendship.
Additionally, the Skillstreaming group increased in social preference such that, on
average, peers rated these children as more liked than disliked.[42 ] Lastly, there were no group differences in parent-reported social skill or autism
severity.
Marro and colleagues[26 ] further explored the effects of SDARI on social knowledge in two concurrent studies.
The first, as part of a laboratory-based randomized controlled efficacy trial (N = 56), participants completed SDARI and another PBSSI delivered for 90 minutes each
week over 10 weeks. Data collected at post-intervention and at 10-week follow-up evinced
improvements in formal social knowledge measures as well as ToM, but no effects were
observed for facial emotion recognition (FER). In the second, uncontrolled study from
Marro and colleagues,[26 ] 13 youth participated in a community-delivered SDARI. Data at post-intervention
showed improvements in participants' FER and ToM, but no effects were observed for
formal social knowledge (i.e., explicit knowledge of social etiquette).[26 ]
Results from these studies indicate that PBSSIs such as SDARI positively impact social
knowledge, ToM, nonverbal communication, social preferences, reciprocated friendships,
and emotion recognition both in the short term and with some maintained effects. However,
across studies there appear to be some mixed results depending on treatment setting
or dosage with regard to social cognition and social communication outcomes. In addition,
SDARI seems at least as effective as more structured, knowledge-based approaches at
promoting reciprocated friendship and is associated with more immediate gains in interaction
and social preference than such approaches. Taken together, results from these studies
suggest that SDARI effectively improves several areas of social communication and
social cognition for autistic youth; however, some gains may be limited to specific
setting or dosage in which SDARI is administered.[7 ]
[26 ]
[37 ]
Imagining Autism
Description. Imagining Autism is a 10-week intervention focused on extending traditional social
skills treatments through immersive learning using imaginative play in autistic school-aged
children (7–12 years).[30 ] Children participated in groups of three to four children with a 1:1 ratio of group
facilitators, for 45 minutes per week within a school. The interventions are conducted
in immersive “pods” (i.e., decorated themed areas) that rotate between five environments
(i.e., under the sea, space, under the city, arctic, and in the forest), twice per
environment throughout the 10 weeks. Interventionists craft and facilitate a story
based on the theme, giving opportunities for the children to spontaneously narrate
and participate in the story. Practitioners encourage turn taking, improvisation,
and “being in the moment” during the experience. All of these activities are designed
to facilitate imagination, social communication, and interactive play.[30 ]
Current empirical evidence. One study has been conducted to examine the feasibility of the Imagining Autism protocol
using an uncontrolled pre-post design. Results from this study in a group of 18 autistic
children report preliminary evidence for improving social communication and social
cognition outcomes. It is noteworthy that modules 1 to 3 from the ADOS-2[43 ] were administered to the participants based on their language level in the study.
No statistically significant differences were noted between pretest and posttest scores
on the communication or creative/play subdomains of the ADOS-2 for all children. For
children who had minimal or limited language abilities (i.e., modules 1 and 2 on the
ADOS-2), the reciprocal social interaction subdomain of the ADOS-2 was not significant.
However, for children who had flexible, verbally fluent language (i.e., who completed
module 3 on the ADOS-2), significant pre-post improvements were noted in the reciprocal
social interaction subdomain. Additionally, when examining single subjects through
a confidence interval analysis (one statistical method for pilot studies), the communication
subdomain (n = 4) and reciprocal social interaction subdomain (n = 7) evinced improvements from pre- to post-intervention. Also, when examining ADOS-2
severity scores, there was a decrease in autism severity scores over time (from pre-
to post-intervention and from post-intervention to follow-up) for those with flexible
language. Participants also demonstrated significant improvements in FER between pre-intervention
and follow-up (but not from pre to post). Moreover, parent- and teacher-report reflects
some positive improvements in ASD symptom severity. Furthermore, parent-reported adaptive
behavior, specifically in communication and socialization domains, showed a significant
increase from pretest to posttest ([Table 1 ]).[30 ]
Taken together, findings suggest that Imagining Autism results in improvements in
socialization, communication, emotion recognition, and improvements in ASD symptoms.
However, there were no improvements in the creativity and play skills, and areas that
evinced some improvements may be limited to a subset of participants or show a delayed
effect. Although this is the first study to examine Imagining Autism, preliminary
results suggest that the intervention is associated with reductions in the social
communicative symptoms of ASD; clearly, replication and extension of this study using
more rigorous designs (to control threats to internal validity and the use of larger
more diverse samples) is warranted.[30 ]
Hunter Heartbeat Method
Description. The Hunter Heartbeat Method is a 1 hour per week, 10-week intervention that utilizes
drama games using Shakespeare's The Tempest to promote the development of social communication skills in children 10 to 14 years
old.[44 ] Sessions are conducted in small groups (six to eight children) with a 1:3 ratio
of children to facilitators to ensure each child is receiving individualized support
and feedback. Intervention is conducted in school, often using school auditoriums
to make use of the “stage” for the drama games. Each session begins with a “heartbeat
circle,” which marks the beginning of the activity and is intended to allow children
time to orient to the social and instructional environment. Following the “heartbeat
circle,” children form dyads to practice and receive feedback on that day's game.
Children “perform” the various games for the group, with each session consisting of
five to seven games to reflect the plot of The Tempest. Following the games, children return to the floor for a “goodbye heartbeat” circle.
Throughout the intervention, facilitators work to encourage affective imitation, turn
taking, personal space, and eye contact. Additionally, facial emotional recognition,
pragmatic language skills, humor, and improvisation are targeted and integrated into
1:1 practice sessions and the theater performances.[44 ]
Current empirical evidence. One study to date, using an uncontrolled pre-post design, has examined the feasibility
of the Hunter Heartbeat Method in small group of 14 children in school setting. Children
who participated in the intervention displayed improvements in socialization, expressive
language, relationships (assessed through standardized parent-reported measure; [Table 1 ]), and pragmatic language abilities (assessed via clinician-administered standardized
measure; [Table 1 ]).[45 ] However, there were no significant improvements from pre- to post-intervention in
FER for the whole sample.[45 ] Of note, half of the participants (n = 7) had high FER scores at pre-intervention, such that significant amelioration
may not have been possible due to a ceiling effect. Descriptive analysis excluding
these seven participants revealed that for many of the remaining participants, the
scores at post-test were improved, with four participants showing increased FER scores.
Furthermore, anecdotal feedback from parents (as part of a social validity questionnaire)
suggested increase in socialization in the home setting alluding to generalization
of treatment gains ([Table 1 ]).[45 ]
Results from this study suggest the Hunter Heartbeat Program is associated with improved
social communication in autistic children, specifically, in clinician- and parent-reported
socialization, expressive language, relationships, and pragmatic language. Although
the increase in pre-post scores was not significant (which may be due to limited statistical
power owing to small sample size), this provides preliminary evidence that the Heartbeat
Method may evince positive changes in FER for some participants. These results are
from a single study examining the therapeutic potential of this intervention. More
rigorous study designs using larger samples are needed to further explore efficacy
and effectiveness.[44 ]
[45 ]
Social Competence Intervention Program
Description. SCIP is a 16-session, manualized intervention program that focuses on improving social
communication in a naturalistic setting, in children and adolescents aged 8 to 14
years, using drama-based peer interactions (e.g., games, story dramatization).[27 ] The initial sessions focus on intrapersonal affective ToM (i.e., understanding one's own emotions), followed by interpersonal affective ToM (i.e., understanding others' emotions).[11 ]
[12 ]
[13 ]
[14 ] The first seven sessions cover topics like establishing group cohesion, emotional
knowledge, focusing attention, facial expression and body language, vocal cues, and
putting several cues together.[27 ] The participants engage in games like “Say It With a Feeling” in which a single
statement is expressed with varied emotions in multiple trials and the “audience”
guesses the specific emotion. This exercise is intended to help participants better
understanding subtle nuances of nonverbal communication (e.g., tone, prosody, facial
expressions) and to support the reading of emotional state of others. A game called
“Jell-O Room” is intended to support the understanding of body language, which is
considered an essential component of social communication. During the “Jell-O Room”
game, participants navigate a room while improvising, as if the room is inundated
with emotions or tangible substances, using body language to express the feeling state.
The next five sessions focus on cognitive ToM which refers to the ability to infer mental states (e.g., thoughts, belief) of
self or others.[13 ]
[46 ] The emphasis of these sessions is particularly on nonverbal cues. A drama-based
teaching methodology called process drama is incorporated in this phase where potential interactions in a social situation
are divided into less complex stages and relevant emotions are discussed in the context
of nonverbal cues. In one such activity, “detective agency,” participants enact a
scenario where detectives (the participating children) and crime witnesses (the interventionists)
adopt their respective roles to unravel a mystery plot. The last four sessions focus
on teaching and honing skills related to functionally responding to peers in a social
setting.[27 ]
[47 ]
Current empirical evidence. One study to date has assessed the effects of SCIP in a group of 34 children with
ASD, nonverbal learning disability, or attention deficit hyperactivity (ADHD) split
into a treatment group (n = 18) and WLC group (n = 16).[27 ] Pre- and posttreatment observations of social interactions were conducted in a school
setting for a 20-minute time interval for 17 (43.6%) of the total participants (8
from the treatment group). The treatment group (for which 11 had ASD) showed improvement
in explicitly observed social behavior, specifically an increase in positive interactions
and decrease in solitary play in comparison to the control group, but no significant
group differences were observed for parent-reported social skills and social withdrawal
(assessed via a parent-reported measure of social and behavioral adjustment), or FER
(assessed through a computer-based measure of receptive nonverbal cues).[45 ] Participants in the treatment group and their parents completed a researcher-developed
semistructured social validity interview assessing perceived efficacy of the intervention.
Qualitative analyses revealed that parents reported positive outcomes, such as improved
self-regulation, increased empathy, increased facial expressions congruent with mood,
and better reciprocity in social interactions. Additionally, parents reported improvements
in their children's understanding of nonverbal cues and body language. Furthermore,
child participants reported positive changes in self-efficacy to decode nonverbal
cues to comprehend feelings, initiate friendships, and the acquired ability to decipher
incongruence in facial and verbally stated affect in others.[27 ]
Taken together, SCIP has shown promising results for improving social communication
outcomes in a preliminary pilot study. The intervention evinced amelioration in observed
social behavior in the treatment group. Notably, gains were not observed in FER and
parent-reported social skills via standardized measures, but qualitative analyses
revealed treatment gains in ToM and socioemotional reciprocity. Encouragingly, the
intervention displays some generalization in treatment gains outside of the treatment
setting as per parent reports, which is an integral aspect in maintaining the skills.
Other Intervention Approaches
Several other PBSSIs that are not theater- or drama-based were identified in the search
but were not included in the review due to alternative approaches or differing definitions
of SSIs. Many of these interventions were conducted in a virtual environment where
children participated in collaborative game play.[48 ]
[49 ]
[50 ] Similar to in-person PBSSIs, participants role-played, played virtual games with
peers (i.e., building together, completing puzzles),[48 ]
[49 ] or participated in unstructured interaction with peers in a virtual reality environment
meant to simulate in vivo social experiences.[49 ]
[51 ] These interventions were associated with improvements in social interaction, emotion
recognition, and social communication skills.[48 ]
[49 ]
[50 ] Other interventions were focused on younger groups of children and were strictly
unstructured play environments, such as playground-based play,[52 ] participation in team activities (e.g., basketball),[53 ] or play environments mediated by parents.[54 ] Playground-based interventions focused on allowing children to participate in free
play with a playground facilitator who encouraged group play and provided group activities
focused on the children's specific interests (i.e., a bug hunt).[52 ] Another play-based intervention utilized basketball to encourage communication and
social interaction in the children through the sport.[53 ] Lastly, the youngest group of children participated in unstructured play with their
parent in dyads where they were encouraged to communicate and interact with their
caregiver.[54 ] These play-based interventions were associated with improvements in social behaviors
such as social interactions, engagement, as well as improvements in repetitive behaviors.[52 ]
[53 ]
[54 ] Furthermore, numerous music and dance therapy approaches measured supplemental effects
of social skills improvements, with both identified studies showing general improvements
in social communication skills.[36 ]
[55 ] However, the goals of music and dance interventions are often not specific to improving
social skills competence; rather, these effects are incidental improvements as these
interventions tend to focus on general improvements in social and emotional well-being.
Discussion
This review article identified five TBSSIs that have data regarding effectiveness
or efficacy in improving social communication and social cognition in individuals
with ASD in the extant literature: SENSE Theatre, SDARI, Imagining Autism, Hunter
Heartbeat Method, and SCIP. In terms of outcomes related to social communication,
these TBSSIs appear to be promising for improving socialization, social knowledge,
peer relationships, peer liking, and social reciprocity. For instance, SENSE Theatre
has demonstrated effectiveness in increasing global social communication symptoms,
social awareness, and social interaction. Specifically, participants who completed
the treatment displayed increased verbal involvement with peers and displayed increased
solicited group play.[28 ]
[29 ]
[33 ] Still, results for daily social communication, functioning, and self-directed play
were mixed, with some studies suggesting improvements, while others did not. Additionally,
SENSE Theatre did not result in improvements in amount of eye contact, which is sometimes
considered an important aspect of social communication.[39 ] Lack of improvements in some areas of social communication as well as the presence
of mixed results could be related to smaller sample sizes in some studies that may
lack power, or differences in treatment setting (i.e., afterschool vs. summer camp)
and length or dosage (e.g., 2 vs. 10 weeks; 2 vs. 4 hour/week).
Research comparing SDARI to a didactic, knowledge-based SSI (Skillstreaming) indicated
that even though both approaches were beneficial in improving social outcomes as reported
by direct observation (for peer interaction, peer liking, and reciprocal friendships),
SDARI showed immediate gains at post-intervention in these outcome variables compared
with Skillstreaming.[7 ] It may be that intrinsically motivating activities of SDARI and the reinforcing
nature of the peer interactions within the groups, compared with the didactic training
in Skillstreaming, explains more immediate gains in peer liking. In addition to the
aforementioned social outcomes, SDARI has been shown to be effective in increasing
assertive social behavior, emotion recognition, ToM, and explicit social knowledge,
although some of these effects may depend on treatment setting and/or dosage.[7 ]
[26 ]
[37 ] Notably, results from a 6-week follow-up also suggest that SDARI is associated with
reduced social problems, indicating lasting effects of group involvement. Researchers
theorized that the combination of assertion and improved emotion recognition ability
may lead to greater social confidence and accuracy in interpreting social situations,
which in turn decrease social problems over time. However, some of these changes did
not reflect improvements in broader parent-reported social behavior, suggesting the
need to more fully examine maintenance or generalization of effects beyond treatment
settings.
There is tentative evidence for additional TBSSI approaches, including Imagining Autism,
which is shown to facilitate social communication and socialization in children with
ASD.[30 ] However, the differential improvements noted in the participants suggest that changes
in the assessed constructs may not be salient enough to be immediately observable
by caregivers posttreatment. Alternatively, due to a small sample size, this feasibility
study may have been underpowered to detect changes in some of the measures.[30 ] A small sample of children who participated in the Hunter Heartbeat Method displayed
improvements in socialization, expressive language, relationships, and pragmatic language
abilities, indicating preliminary effectiveness in treating social communication deficits.[45 ] Similarly, SCIP seems to show effects in improved social interaction and decreased
solitary play as assessed by observations in the naturalistic setting (school) and
coding of this observational data, while some of the parent-reported standardized
measures (e.g., withdrawal and social skills domains) did not indicate pre- and post-changes.
Likewise, parent- and child self-report gathered by semistructured interviews revealed
a positive impact of intervention on social functioning (e.g., self-regulation, cognitive
and affective ToM, empathy).[27 ] These differences can be attributed to methods variance related to assessing informant
versus direct performance. Specifically, some of the measures may not be sensitive
enough to assess changes in the constructs as compared with other measures.
Various TBSSIs appear to be promising approaches in terms of outcomes related to social
cognition as well. SENSE Theatre has been reviewed in the context of several empirical
studies examining improvements in both broader social cognition skills and more specific
skills such as memory for faces and broad measurement of ToM. Specifically, SENSE
Theatre produced mixed outcomes in contextual (i.e., relating to a social context
or experience) versus verbal ToM (i.e., communication of internal emotions). In one
study,[28 ] greater improvements in contextual ToM (i.e., relating to a social context or experience)
were noted in the treatment group compared with waitlist control group.[28 ]
[33 ] However, results of another study[29 ] indicated amelioration in verbal ToM skills, as participants were able to communicate
mental states to others, but not in contextual ToM in terms of emotionally relating
to the social context of the event. These inconsistent findings could be due to the
lack of specificity of the assessment used to measure the construct. For example,
the use of more sensitive measures or a multifaceted evaluation of ToM skills, like
the inclusion of false-belief understanding and/or pretend play skills, might lead
to different results.[26 ]
[29 ] Additionally, consistent improvements were reported for face identification for
memory for delayed recall, while mixed results were reported for immediate recall of faces. Furthermore, EEG-indexed neural evidence of memory for face was
also shown in the treatment group. These findings suggest some differential treatment
effects on social cognitive skills, which could relate to several factors, including
varied session and treatment length. Lastly, some areas in which SENSE Theatre did
not evince significant improvements (e.g., affect recognition) were assessed in two
small groups of children and have not been examined in subsequent studies with larger
group participation.[33 ]
Evidence for SDARI suggests that gains in social knowledge can be implicitly acquired
by TBSSIs in autistic individuals.[26 ] It is noteworthy that even in the absence of didactic instruction, participants
demonstrated gains in the higher ToM skills (e.g., sarcasm) and emotion identification,
which are areas in which many autistic people require increased support. This suggests
that a naturalistic treatment setting may be more conducive to improving emotion identification
skills, but it also suggests that less-controlled, lower fidelity delivery of interventions
may differentially impact treatment effectiveness (as demonstrated by lack of social
knowledge gains in the second study). Another notable finding is that effects may
vary by dosage and/or format of the intervention delivered; in one study, which was
laboratory-based, participants demonstrated improvement in social knowledge and ToM
but not affect recognition, while in another study, which was community-based, gains in ToM
and affect recognition were noted, but not formal social knowledge. Differential gains in social cognition, emotion recognition,
and ToM may be related to the setting or dosage in which SDARI is administered, suggesting
that these factors should be a focus of future research.
Finally, the two studies examining Imagining Autism and the Hunter Heartbeat Method
assessed social cognition in smaller groups of participants and used non-experimental
designs. Preliminary results from the Imagining Autism protocol showed that intervention
was associated with improvements in social cognition. While FER improvements were
not uniformly present at posttreatment, children presented with low pre-intervention
scores demonstrated improvements at follow-up, suggesting this intervention may produce
a delayed effect in higher level processing and recognition of emotion.[30 ] There were no significant social cognitive improvements following the Hunter Heartbeat
Program, suggesting that some TBSSIs may differentially impact broader social communication
but not specific aspects of social cognition that may underlie such changes despite
similar measurement of the construct.[45 ]
Taken together, the evidence-base for TBSSIs is relatively sparse but growing. Extant
literature is largely characterized by less rigorous designs with small sample sizes.
Still, the evidence that is accumulating provides some proof of concept and justifies
further examination of their therapeutic potential to establish causal linkages, identify
active ingredients of treatment, and determine whether and which participant characteristics
predict success with intervention. In general, TBSSIs appear to be promising for supporting
in social communication outcomes including increased social interactions, reciprocal
communication, and improved peer relationships. Furthermore, by providing socially
safe and interactive spaces for practicing social communication with peers, constructs
of social cognition appear to be positively affected by TBSSIs, including improved
emotion recognition and ToM, which results in improved integration of learned and
applied skills. However, many of the findings related to these outcomes were inconsistent
across parent- and teacher-reported changes in behaviors, suggesting some of the improvements
may not generalize across contexts. In light of the aforementioned small sample sizes,
many of the studies reviewed may not been sufficiently powered to detect effects.
This gains importance when considering the effect size estimates provided in the literature
for some outcome variables of interest (see [Table 1 ]). Specifically, SENSE Theatre produced consistently large effects of improvements
in social cognition (e.g., facial identification memory), whereas improvements in
social communication were small to medium (e.g., ToM, group play) and inconsistent
across studies.[28 ]
[29 ]
[33 ]
[39 ] SDARI produced medium-to-large effects for several social communication measures
(e.g., participant interactions).[7 ] Imagining Autism produced large effects for both measured social communication (i.e.,
reciprocal communication) and social cognition (i.e., FER) for some participants.[30 ] These findings highlight the variability in outcomes between approaches, and underscore
the importance of more closely examining effect sizes and the dosage required to establish
a therapeutic effect, as well as the active ingredients of treatment that may be responsible
for these changes.
Theoretical Considerations on Effects of TBSSIs
These findings suggest that mechanisms for social learning in youth with ASD may be
mediated by specific content as well as individual differences, and can be supported
through specific performance-based techniques when provided in a supportive context.
This is contrary to the notion or assumption that they do not know the discrete steps
of social interaction and cannot implicitly learn about social norms, rules, or conventions,
unless they are taught explicitly using more traditional methods (e.g., like a classroom)
in a didactic social skills curriculum. Indeed, in real life, social situations rarely
present themselves in an explicit and structured fashion, so it takes significantly
more in vivo processes to use their available social cognition. As reviewed here,
TBSSIs, which prioritize in vivo opportunities for social engagement, may help youth
with ASD learn knowledge-based aspects of social cognition implicitly, rather than
exclusively explicitly, and provide future directions to support learning of social
skills.
Several prominent hypotheses may help explain deficits in social communication and
social cognition in individuals with ASD, which, in turn, may form the basis of specific
theoretical models of social functioning that may explain the effectiveness and efficacy
of TBSSIs. For instance, Dissanayake and Macintosh[56 ] presented the Hacking Hypothesis , which states that some autistic individuals may “hack” out the mechanisms (i.e.,
rote learn) required to perform on the standard tasks (e.g., false-belief tasks in
standard ToM assessments), which are not generalized to the real-world setting. That
is, individuals with ASD may “hack out” superficial knowledge of social mores and
prosocial behavior, and, therefore, may appear socially competent (e.g., in a knowledge-based
SSI).[56 ] However, in line with the distinction that Gresham[57 ] made, those acquiring social knowledge (i.e., knowledge about the correct behavior
in each social situation) may still demonstrate deficits in social performance (i.e.,
lacking the ability to apply social knowledge in real-life situations with peers or
adults). It is plausible that TBSSIs provide social learning opportunities that go
beyond traditional SSI for actually practicing and building skills in how to apply
social knowledge. Indeed, theater-based activities inherently provide an opportunity
for social interaction and engagement with others through various techniques, such
as acting games that target perceiving and interpreting information from others and
responding to it, and improve methods that enhance flexibility and imagination.
In a related vein, Jeste and Nelson[58 ] presented the Attentional Hypothesis (rooted in social motivation theory), which posits that individuals with ASD lack
an innate preference to attend to social stimuli. Due to the heterogeneity of ASD,
the variation in this type of preference may be prognosticative of social functioning.
Similarly, Chevallier and colleagues[59 ] advanced the Social Motivation Theory which posits that individuals with ASD may not be motivated by social stimuli or
social engagement and, therefore, selectively attend to environmental stimuli, which
results in impairment in social functioning. However, the assumption that autistic
individuals present with diminished social motivation may fail to account for the
diverse ways in which autistic individuals express their social interest. It is important,
therefore, to consider factors other than social motivation that could limit an autistic
individual's ability to engage in behaviors that are typically thought to indicate
social interest.[60 ] Thus, Jaswal and Akhtar[61 ] argued that attempting to instruct a behavior that is “conventionally interpreted
as indicating social interest,” such as making an eye contact, could paradoxically
backfire when autistic children find engaging in such behaviors aversive with exaggerated
bids from adults. Therefore, rather than didactically teaching autistic children to
look other people directly in the eye, it may be important to provide natural and
intrinsically motivating opportunities for joint attention and shifting gaze in the
context of collaborative activities that provide context for shared positive experiences.
Taken together, the above hypotheses may form the basis of specific models of social
functioning that could potentially explain therapeutic effects of TBSSIs. Klin and
collegues[61 ] elucidated the Enactive Mind Model which highlights the development of social cognition
via the interaction of the individual within their environment. Furthermore, Beauchamp
and Anderson[62 ] stated that according to the Developmental Biopsychosocial Socio-cognitive Integration
of Abilities (SOCIAL) model, social competence is determined by an interplay of cognitive
(e.g., attention, memory, executive function), environmental (or external), and individual
(or internal) factors. The variation in cognitive factors mediates the process of
acquiring social competence. These models are in sync with the neuroconstructivist
framework,[63 ]
[64 ] which underscores the importance of considering a multidirectional interplay among
the developmental domains. Indeed, multiple domains of functioning (e.g., language,
cognition) are likely to synergistically influence social functioning in autistic
individuals. This process may be further limited in individuals with ASD due to differences
in social motivation and selective attention to social stimuli. However, improvements
in social cognition evinced by TBSSIs are consistent with this notion that social
cognitive processes are a result or embodiment of experiences of how individuals respond and act in social situations. These arguments
underscore the theoretical potency of interventions providing in vivo opportunities
for practicing and processing social situations to develop social competence more
effectively.[7 ]
[16 ]
[20 ]
[26 ]
[27 ]
[28 ]
[29 ]
[30 ]
[33 ]
[37 ]
[40 ]
Limitations and Future Directions
Limitations and Future Directions
There are several limitations in the current literature surrounding TBSSIs. First,
the identified TBSSIs report findings of studies which recruited autistic individuals
with IQ > 70 (except for Imagining Autism) in community- or clinic-based treatment
settings.[7 ]
[26 ]
[27 ]
[28 ]
[29 ] While not explicitly stated, most participants were presumed to have language sufficient
to allow their participation in the TBSSIs, which suggests at least fluent expressive
language. Future research in TBSSIs should seek to adapt to these interventions for
children with more limited language abilities.[65 ] Furthermore, these interventions tended to employ small sample sizes and included
mostly adolescent males, who are predominantly white, limiting generalizability of
these findings. Consequently, it will be important to expand research on these interventions
to include larger, more diverse samples of individuals with a wider range of cognitive
abilities, to examine the generalizability of the treatment effectiveness/efficacy.
Moreover, when a TBSSI requires specialized training in addition to reviewing the
instructional manual, it can be difficult for researchers to conduct studies to empirically
validate the approach with high treatment fidelity. Thus, future research can address
these limitations and propose methods to bridge the research-to-practice gap.
Most of the studies do not have a more rigorous, empirically sound control group (other
than a WLC). This design feature is often critical in establishing a causal relationship
between the intervention and the outcome. Future studies should include RCTs with
larger and more culturally diverse samples to assess efficacy, along with single-subject
experimental designs, to examine intervention when delivered in a variety of community-based
and clinical settings. Likewise, future reviews and synthesis of this literature should
evaluate the evidentiary quality of the studies reporting on the efficacy or effectiveness
of each intervention type. Notwithstanding the need of effective interventions for
older autistic individuals—who have unique social challenges like maintaining interpersonal
relationships and employment—there is dearth of psychosocial interventions for young
adults.[66 ] Therefore, future studies might focus on TBSSIs adapted to address the unique social
challenges of older individuals with ASD.
In another vein, future studies should make use of more sensitive measures of the
various constructs of interests (e.g., ToM; social knowledge), including multimodal
assessment methods which include self-report measures, direct observation, and multiple
informants.[67 ] It would be especially helpful to assess both proximal, more immediate intervention
goals (e.g., based on outlined objectives of the intervention) and more distal outcomes
(e.g., social communication and social cognition), as well as evaluate the quality
of such evidence. This is needed to establish sound theoretical bases of intervention
and provide further support for effects of these approaches. Because language plays
a critical role in ToM development and the acquisition of social knowledge,[68 ]
[69 ] the use of assessment methods that will not mask the actual abilities due to language
impairment will be especially important. In addition, most studies reviewed here did
not assess nor control for confounding variables (e.g., use of psychotropic medications,
participation in concomitant therapies). Therefore, future studies should examine
how these variables predict treatment outcomes for youth participating in TBSSIs.
Finally, future research should evaluate the unique profiles of autistic individuals
who may benefit from TBSSIs so that interventions can be personalized. This could
be accomplished using single-subject design, or by examination of emerging profiles
via meta-analyses or larger-scale studies that allow for statistically parsing heterogeneity
in outcomes using person-centered approaches (e.g., latent profile analysis). Indeed,
Lerner and colleagues[16 ] outlined the mechanisms of change underlying the amelioration of social functioning
in individuals with ASD in psychosocial interventions such as TBSSIs. Future studies
may also examine variables that mediate and moderate treatment outcomes. Variables
worthy of consideration include, but are not limited to, the quality of the therapeutic
alliance between clinicians and participants[70 ]; the degree of an individual's social motivation, social knowledge, or executive
function skills at pre-intervention; and the presence of comorbid diagnoses.[16 ]
Clinical Implications
This review examined the therapeutic potential of TBSSIs for improving social communication
and social cognition in youth with ASD. Although some interventions are in the initial
stages of research, there is emerging evidence that these interventions may be successfully
implemented in educational, clinical, and community settings. In the future, dissemination
and increasing accessibility through training and implementation of manualized treatment
protocols will be crucial next steps for maximizing the impact of these treatments.
These interventions provide individuals with ASD treatment in a less structured and
perhaps more comfortable and collaborative environment to practice social interaction
and social communication skills without stigma that may accompany more traditional,
didactic learning environment. Furthermore, these interventions are usually delivered
in a group setting, which can reduce the time-burden often experienced by service
providers, and crucially, providing opportunities for participants to receive real-time
reinforcement in a supported environment. Additionally, TBSSIs may have supplemental
effects on cooccurring psychiatric symptoms in ASD, such as anxiety symptoms,[38 ]
[71 ] thus providing an important avenue toward increasing effectiveness of treatment
and reducing burden for both the families and providers.
Summary and Conclusion
The current review examined the bourgeoning research in theater-based SSI approaches
for supporting social communication and social cognition in autistic youth. Taken
together, there is a promising evidence-base for approaches in improving several aspects
of social communication and social cognition, including increased peer interactions
and reciprocal friendships, as well as ToM and emotion recognition. However, the effects
of intervention seem to vary considerably depending on dosage and/or format of the
intervention delivered, and there is limited information whether effects are maintained
following the intervention. Additionally, owing to the relatively recent development
of some approaches, much of this evidence relies on preliminary studies with less
rigorous designs in their initial stages. These interventions should seek to continue
to explore effectiveness and efficacy with more methodological rigor and with larger,
more diverse samples. Moreover, further examination of active ingredients and mechanisms
of change will be a vital step toward maximizing effects of these approaches in supporting
social communication and cognition in this population.