Subscribe to RSS
DOI: 10.1055/a-1973-3038
Listening with an Ageing Brain – a Cognitive Challenge
Article in several languages: deutsch | English- Abstract
- 1. Introduction
- 2. Cognition and speech understanding
- 3. Age-related hearing loss
- 4. Hearing disorders in frequent neurodegnerative diseases in higher ages
- 5. Correlation of hearing loss and cognitive impairment
- 6. Can treatment of hearing loss reduce cognitive impairment?
- 7. Outlook
- Literatur
Abstract
Hearing impairment has been recently identified as a major modifiable risk factor for cognitive decline in later life and has been becoming of increasing scientific interest. Sensory and cognitive decline are connected by complex bottom-up and top-down processes, a sharp distinction between sensation, perception, and cognition is impossible. This review provides a comprehensive overview on the effects of healthy and pathological aging on auditory as well as cognitive functioning on speech perception and comprehension, as well as specific auditory deficits in the 2 most common neurodegenerative diseases in old age: Alzheimer disease and Parkinson syndrome. Hypotheses linking hearing loss to cognitive decline are discussed, and current knowledge on the effect of hearing rehabilitation on cognitive functioning is presented. This article provides an overview of the complex relationship between hearing and cognition in old age.
#
Key words
age related hearing loss - presbycusis - central auditory processing disorder - neurocognitive disorder - Alzheimer disease - Parkinson syndrome - hearing rehabilitation1. Introduction
Successful communication in complex listening situations requires not only the detection of the target signal and the segregation of the scenario into different sound sources. The listener must also track who is speaking, grasp the meaning of the statement, memorize and compare it with already existing knowledge, suppress irrelevant interfering signals, formulate an own response in parallel and execute it at the right time. Longer conversations in groups require the integration of new information with already expressed contents of each speaker while again and again the attention switches between the persons involved.
This means that in order to assess and use the information contained in spoken language, a fluent and swiftly functioning integrative system of perceptual and cognitive processes is required. Both the auditory and cognitive systems are subject to typical aging processes; and with higher age, the incidence of neurodegenerative diseases increases, sometimes having a considerable influence on the ability to communicate. In recent years, hearing disorders have increasingly become the focus of scientific research as a potentially modifiable risk factor for neurocognitive impairment in an aging society. In this review, hypotheses on the causal relationship will be presented as well as specific auditory impairments in the context of the most common neurodegenerative disorders of the elderly. Finally the effect of hearing rehabilitation will be discussed.
#
2. Cognition and speech understanding
2.1 Definition and domains
Cognition (Latin: cognoscere=to recognize, to experience, to perceive) is a collective term for processes of reception, processing, storage, and retrieval of information as well as their results (knowledge, attitudes, beliefs, expectations). These processes can take place consciously, e. g. when solving tasks, or unconsciously, e. g. when forming opinions [1]. Human cognitive skills include, among others, processes of perception, attention, learning and memory, thinking, but also recognition of emotions, and control of one’s own behavior. The ability to use these skills to solve problems, adapt to new situations, and interact effectively with the environment is referred to in psychology as “intelligence” (Latin intelligentia=cognition, intellect). While Cattell’s intelligence model distinguished only between fluid intelligence (innate, experience-independent ability to reason and solve problems) and crystalline intelligence (predominantly culture-dependent ability to apply acquired knowledge), nowadays the Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) model is considered the one that most comprehensively describes the structure of intelligence [2]. It includes 16 factors from the areas of acquired knowledge, thinking ability, processing speed, memory, sensory processing, psychomotor skills, and kinesthetics and serves as the basis for the most widely used intelligence tests.
For the diagnosis of neurocognitive disorders, the “Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders – DSM-5” [3] defines 6 cognitive domains on which the diagnostic criteria are based and which can be assessed in standardized neuropsychological testing ([Table 1]).
Cognitive domain |
Subdomains |
|||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Complex attention |
Permanent attention |
Didived attention |
Selective attention |
Processing rate |
||
Executive functions |
Planning |
Decision making |
Working memory |
Exploiting feedback/correting errors |
Acting against habits/behavioral inhibition |
Mental flexibility |
Learning and memory |
Immediate memory* |
Short-term memory (including free recall, recall with cue stimuli, and recognition) |
Ultra long-term memory (semantic and autobiographical) |
Implicit (procedural) learning |
||
Speech |
Speech production (including naming, word finding, word fluency, grammar and syntax) |
Speech comprehension |
||||
Perceptive-motor |
Visuo perception |
Visuo construction |
Perceptive-motor |
Practice |
Gnosis |
|
Social cognition |
Recognizing emotions |
Theory of mind (ability to observe the Erkennen von Emotionen Theory of Mind (ability to pay attention to another person's state of mind or experience) |
*is sometimes included in the term of working memory.
#
2.3 Normal cognitive aging
Cognitive processes are subject to chronological aging processes to varying degrees and are highly associated with the loss of everyday functioning, onset of dementia, and general mortality [4] [5]. It is well known that basal, knowledge-independent “fluid” functions show a greater age decline than lifelong acquired “crystalline” knowledge, which can still show growth into old age [6]. A persons’ intelligence is seen as the result of function or knowledge build-up, loss, and compensation mechanisms. This means in order to maintain cognitive performance as fluid abilities are lost, we rely more and more on already established, automated crystalline processes to accomplish tasks [6] [7]. Research continues to address the extent to which training can counteract functional loss and the importance of individual cognitive domains in this process. In a large cross-sectional study on 48,537 subjects and evaluation of normative values of standardized IQ and memory tests, Hartsthorne and Germine were able to show that there is greater heterogeneity with regard to the time of maximum functional capacity between the individual domains than previously assumed [8]: short-term memory and processing speed reach maximum values already in the teenage years, working memory peaks in young adulthood with the onset of decline in the 30ies. Peak performance in e. g. vocabulary and emotion recognition, on the other hand, is reached only in the middle age and maintained over a much longer period of several years. As an explanation for individual performance differences, however, non-specific age effects, such as a general slowing, must be considered in addition to these domain- and function-specific changes. Recent long-term studies indicate that approximately 30-50% of individual differences in age progression are due to a “general factor” [9].
In addition to significantly reduced general processing speed and working memory compared to younger people, loss of executive functions and episodic memory occur in older age [10] [11]. Morphologically, changes are seen in the middle temporal lobe (episodic memory) and the prefrontal/striatal system (executive functions) [12]. Neurodegenerative diseases such as Alzheimer’s disease or Parkinson’s syndrome affect these areas to varying degrees and lead to specific functional deficits.
#
2.4 Cognitive reserve
People of about the same age with similar central changes, e. g., in the context of a neurodegenerative disease but also in the course of normal aging, may nevertheless vary considerably in their clinical symptoms and cognitive performance. To explain this observation, the concept of cognitive reserve was introduced [13]. It refers to the ability to compensate for newly occurred damage and to maintain existing functions by using alternative neuronal networks [14]. Both congenital and acquired or environmental factors (e. g., intelligence, educational level, physical activity, recreational and social activities) play a role. Differences in cognitive reserve are also considered as an explanation for the individual impact of sensory impairment (e. g., hearing loss) in higher age [15].
#
2.5 Information processing model and cognitive concepts in relation with hearing and speech comprehension
From a cognition-psychology perspective, spoken communication can be understood as a process of information processing: The incoming stimulus is perceived by the sensory system, processed, and finally leads to a reaction ([Fig 1], adapted from [16]). This complex process depends the properties of the incoming stimulus (bottom-up) and is also influenced by cognitive processes (top-down). In the theoretical model of Wingfield and Tun [17] ([Fig 2]), the interactive roles of peripheral, central, cognitive, and linguistic factors to speech understanding are illustrated in more detail: In the periphery, the sensory system must receive the spectral and temporal cues of the speech signal and pass them on to the central auditory pathway for further processing with as little interference as possible. In the next stage of central auditory processing (perceptual system), binaural information is encoded in addition to spectral and temporal features of the speech signal (especially signal onset and duration). The so-called “object formation”, i. e. the ability to recognize a target signal and to follow it in the presence of competing background noise or speakers, also occurs at this level. This is followed by the linguistic operations of sound analysis and lexical recognition at the word level. Based on syntactic (position of a word in a sentence) and semantic (word meaning) prior knowledge, sentences are captured. The comparison with contextual information (speaker, situation, object, time, etc.) finally enables comprehension within the conversation [18]. The single processing steps are influenced by cognitive abilities or processes such as memory functions (prior knowledge, working memory) and general processing speed, attention, and executive functions (top-down). At the same time, the characteristics of the stimulus (e. g., speech rate, accent, type and number of noise sources, reverberation etc.) determine subsequent processing (bottom-up). Auditory and cognitive processes are so closely intertwined that a sharp separation of “peripheral” and “central” auditory functions does not adequately capture the complexity of speech processing [19]. The typical complaint of the elderly – to hear but to understand poorly – is merely a clinical symptom of normal age-related changes in all sections of this system from the periphery to the cortex, which may be additionally impaired by neurodegenerative diseases.
#
#
3. Age-related hearing loss
3.1 Prevalence and socio-economic consequences
In 2019, according to the WHO, about 1.5 billion people worldwide were affected by hearing loss [20], and 430 million (about 5.5% of the world population) had at least moderate hearing impairment. The WHO expects this number to increase to 700 million people with moderate or higher levels of hearing loss in the better hearing ear by 2050, out of a projected total of 2.5 billion people affected. The individual development of hearing throughout the life span depends on various protective and damaging factors [21]. In addition to genetic, biological, and environmental influences, individual lifestyle (nicotine abuse, diet, noise exposure) also plays a role. Age-related hearing loss (ARHL) represents the greatest socio-economic burden over a lifetime due to its high prevalence in the population. According to current estimates, in 2019 approximately 42% of all people affected by hearing loss were at least 60 years old [20], and the proportion of moderate or higher levels of hearing loss increases exponentially with higher age (prevalence at 60-69 years: 15.4%; more than 90 years: 58.2%). WHO estimates the annual costs due to hearing loss to be approximately $ 980 billion. In recent years, age-related hearing loss has been increasingly identified as a potential risk factor for neurocognitive disorders [22] [23] [24] [25]. Positive effects of audiological rehabilitation with hearing aids for the course of these disorders [26] [27] [28] as well as health-related quality of life [29] are seen. Nevertheleess, in Europe, hearing aids are used by only about 33% of the approximately 57 million people with hearing loss in need of care, although they are widely available [20] [30].
#
3.2 Age-related changes of the peripheral auditory system
Age-related degenerative processes affect both outer and inner hair cells, supporting cells, stria vascularis, and spiral ganglion cells [31] [32] [33] [34] [35] [36]. The pure-tone audiogram typically shows a loss of high frequency hearing [36] [37] [38]. For medical expert reports, DIN EN ISO 7029:2017 should be consulted, which allows estimation of normal hearing for ages between 20 and 80+ years [39] ([Fig 3]). The current 3rd version is based on data from healthy men and women published after 2000. Compared with previous versions, the average hearing loss is lower for all age groups, reflecting changes in living and working conditions.
Based on experience from animal models regarding the underlying etiology, Dubno et al. [37] classified audiometric phenotypes of age-related hearing loss. A low-grade hearing loss up to 1 kHz and rather flat high-frequency hearing loss in indicative of metabolically-induced atrophy and degeneration of the stria vascularis, whereas a steeply declining hearing threshold between 2 and 8 kHz with normal low-frequency hearing indicates a sensory disorder (hair or supporting cell damage).
In the same study [37], approximately 11% of pure-tone audiograms were classified as “older normal”, with an average hearing loss of no more than 20 dB HL in the high-frequency range. Nevertheless, elderlies with normal pure-tone audiograms also report hearing difficulties and tinnitus [40] [41]. For this hidden hearing loss (HHL), different pathophysiological mechanisms have been discussed in recent years [42] [43] [44]. In addition to disturbances of the afferent synapse of the inner hair cells (cochlear synaptopathy [35] [42] [43] [45] [46]), demyelination processes (temporary loss of cochlear Schwann cells [47] or in the context of demyelinating neuropathy [48]), and persistent dysfunction of the outer hair cells [49] [50] have been described. These changes lead to impaired transmission of temporal and spectral fine structure [51], especially of rapid signal changes as well as signal duration. The phonetic contrasts necessary for accurate word recognition decrease, which manifests in reduced speech understanding, especially in noisy environments, even before high-frequency hearing loss is detected in the puretone audiogram.
Amplitude changes of wave I of the early auditory evoked potential elicited by suprathreshold stimulation [34] [46] [51] or an altered SP/AP amplitude ratio in electrocochleography [52] are discussed as electrophysiologic markers of the disturbed cochlear function.
Age-related changes of the central auditory system
3.2.1 Structural-morphological as well as neurochemical changes
Aging processes affect the entire central auditory pathway from the cochlear nucleus to the auditory cortex (see [53] [54] for a comprehensive overview). Throughout the lifespan, the human cortex is subject to remodeling processes that become visible and measurable due to modern imaging techniques such as magnetic resonance imaging. MR spectroscopy also allows metabolic and neurochemical changes to be detected. In healthy adults, there is a general brain volume reduction with increasing age [55] [56] [57]. Volume changes in gray matter [58] [59] [60] and white matter [60] [61] [62] as well as cortex thickness [58] [63] have been discribed. Regions particularly affected include the temporal lobe, hippocampus [60] [64] [65] and prefrontal cortex [59] [61] [66] [67]. Lin et al. [68] demonstrated that hearing loss accelerates volume decline in both the total volume and the right temporal lobe. Further studies showed gray matter reductions beyond the age norm in the superior and medial temporal gyrus [69] and superior and medial frontal gyrus [69] [70] [71], primary auditory cortex [72] [73], and occipital lobe and hypothalamus [70]. Diffusion-weighted MR images also showed changes in myelination, fiber density, and axonal parameters in the superior olive complex, lemniscus lateralis, and inferior colliculus [69] [74]. MR spectroscopy has demonstrated dysfunction of GABAergic neurotransmission in the central auditory system of patients with presbycusis [54] [75] [76].
This means that, on one hand, structural changes in the central auditory pathway already occur in the course of normal aging, which can have a negative effect on speech understanding; on the other hand, age-related hearing loss additionally leads to impairment of further areas in the association cortex [77].
#
3.2.2 Changes of central-auditory processing and perception
Structural and neurochemical changes in the central hearing pathway lead to impaired encoding of temporal characteristics of speech. As part of normal aging processes, there are changes in neural timing and precision in speech processing [18] with implications for comprehension of speech both in quiet and in noise. In general, the ability to perceive rapid temporal changes in the speech signal decreases. That is, older people need larger differences or temporally longer features (voice onset time, vowel duration, pauses etc.) to distinguish individual speech sounds [78]. If the speech signal is additionally spectrally altered, these difficulties increase, as several studies with vocoded speech have shown [79] [80]. This is particularly relevant with regard to cochlear implant fitting. Impaired neural encoding of signal onset is also thought to be the cause of greater difficulty for elders to understand speech with altered speed, stress, or rhythm. For example, research by Gordon-Salant et al. demonstrated that older normal-hearing subjects have significantly greater problems with understanding fast speakers or speech with a foreign accent [81] [82].
The ability to separate single speech streams, i. e. to follow a speaker in the presence of noise or competing speakers, also declines with age and has been demonstrated in a multitude of studies [83] [84] [85] [86]. This has been attributed to impaired processing of temporal fine structure as well as perception of brief amplitude changes in the envelope of the speech signal (“listen to the dips”) [87]. In addition, age effects have been demonstrated in the binaural processing of speech signals [88] [89] [90].
A comprehensive review of age-related electrophysiological changes in the central auditory pathway can be found in [91]. Early auditory evoked potentials, especially the so-called frequency following response (FFR) after stimulation with both tone and speech signals, objectify impaired temporal processing at the brainstem level. Depending on the experimental design, late auditory evoked potentials allow both the differential detection of disturbed temporal processing of auditory stimuli at the cortical level, independent of attention and cognition (N1-P2) and the assessment of cognitive processes if the potentials are measured in an event-related manner (P300, N200). Therefore, the latter can also be used to distinguish between normal aging processes, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s dementia [92].
#
3.2.3 Central presbycusis
In English-speaking countries, the described age-related disorders of central processing and perception of auditory information with age-appropriate pure-tone hearing threshold are summarized under the term of central auditory processing disorder (CAPD) or central presbycusis [93] [94]. The disorder is considered to have multifactorial causes, correlations with age-related cognitive disorders are seen, clinically, a sharp separation between cognitive and auditory processing is hardly possible.
The German Society of Phoniatrics and Pediatric Audiology defines auditory processing and perception disorders in more detail: According to the current guideline, the diagnosis of auditory processing and perception disorders should only be made if, at age-appropriate pure-tone hearing thresholds, there are deficits in analysis, differentiation, and identification of time, frequency, and intensity changes of acoustic or auditory speech signals as well as processes of binaural interaction (e. g., for sound localization, lateralization, noise suppression, and summation) and dichotic processing that cannot be better explained by other disorders, such as attention deficits, general cognitive deficits, cross-modality mnestic disorders [95]. The deficits in the auditory domain must be significant compared to language-independent cognitive performance. At the same time, there is a high comorbidity to e. g. disorders of attention. Clinically, it must then be decided, taking into account all findings, which disorder is leading. With regard to differentiation from infantile auditory processing and perception disorder and in view of the usually modality-spanning aging processes, it seems to make sense to rather use the term of “central presbycusis” for disorders of central auditory processing newly occurring in older age.
#
#
3.3 Influence of cognitive processes on speech comprehension
In order to follow a conversation successfully and participate in it, listeners and speakers must not only perceive what is being said and understand the single words even under unfavorable complex conditions (background noise, reverberation, high speech rate, accent, etc.), but also grasp the content in context, compare it with their own prior knowledge, and formulate a response. On the cognitive level, this requires, among other things, keeping one’s attention on the target signal, storing it in working memory, and matching it with long-term memory – as quickly as possible in order to be able to follow the rest of the conversation. Working memory, executive functions, and processing speed are therefore seen as the most important cognitive factors for speech comprehension, especially in noise [96], and a large number of studies have investigated them (e. g., executive functions and attention [97] [98], processing rate and working memory [87]). The importance of auditory and cognitive factors and their interaction for the quality of speech comprehension has been increasingly taken into account in the last 10-20 years, so that the term of “cognitive hearing science” has been established [99].
3.3.1 Inhibition control
In the information processing model by Wingfield and Tun [17] ([Fig 2]), the “attention filter” symbolizes the ability to selectively follow a single signal in the presence of noise or competing speakers and thus to suppress further processing of the non-selected speech streams very early in the process. Disruption of inhibition control, e. g., in the course of normal aging, limits this ability and may thereby impair speech comprehension.
At the word level, perceived phonemes must be matched with the mental lexicon. The success of this lexical process depends on the frequency of occurrence of a word within a language as well as the number of words, with overlapping phonemes (neighborhood density). The Neighborhood Activation Model [100] theorizes that the more frequently a word occurs within a language (high frequency) and the fewer words with overlapping phonemes (low neighborhood density), the easier it is to recognize the word correctly. Accordingly, words with high neighborhood density have more competitors that must be suppressed by the listener to enable correct word retrieval. Research on the neighborhood density effect has shown that in older adults, there is a significant relationship between measures of inhibition control and speech comprehension in noise (e. g., [101]). In addition, with increasing age, frequently occurring competing words are more intrusive, i. e. they are more often misidentified as a target signal [102] [103].
#
3.3.2 Working memory
In cognition psychology, working memory is understood as limited resource that allows information to be kept and processed in immediate memory [104]. In phonological analysis, working memory is considered to play a significant role as an interface to long-term memory. To explain why in some situations speech understanding is effortless while in others increased listening effort is required, Rönnberg et al. developed the Ease of Language Understanding (ELU) model (see [105] for a comprehensive review). The incoming multimodal signal is quickly and automatically matched (within 180-200 ms [16]) with the mental lexicon. If a minimum number of matching phonological attributes is found, the implicit lexical process proceeds rapidly, and the signal is understood. If no match is found, semantic and episodic long-term memory must be explicitly accessed with the aid of working memory to enable language processing. If the input signal is difficult to understand – e. g., due to hearing impairment or unfavorable acoustic environment – it must be held longer in working memory and more cognitive resources must be expended to understand what is being said. Listening effort increases [106]. In particular, a significant dependence on working memory capacity has been shown for speech comprehension in noise, independent of age [107] [108].
#
3.3.3 Significance of the context
Phonological matching can be facilitated by the aid of contextual information, allowing partial compensation for the deficits caused by hearing impairment. Benichow et al. [109], for example, demonstrated that although hearing loss had a significant effect on speech understanding in noise, it decreased with increasing probability of the target word to occur in the context of the sentence. At the same time, both age and cognitive performance (especially working memory as well as processing speed) were significant predictors of speech understanding independent of the amount of contextual information.
Increasing deficits in inhibition control with age may, in turn, contribute to wrongly identify acoustically unintelligible words as utterances that are probable within context [110] [111] [112]. A recent study by van Os et al. [113] revealed that older subjects are also able to rationally adjust their response behavior within a trial and, for example, rely more on the acoustic information than the context when the context offered is misleading.
#
3.3.4 Listening effort
If cognitive resources must be used to understand disturbed speech signal, they are lacking for other processes such as encoding what is heard into memory. The so-called “Framework for Understanding Effortful Listening” (FUEL) [114] describes successful speech comprehension as dependent on the quality of the acoustic stimulus, the demand of the task, and the listener’s motivation to exert the effort necessary to achieve it. Increased listening effort may not only deplete available cognitive resources more rapidly, but also reduce the listener’s motivation to exert that effort at all – even if the utterance itself was correctly understood.
#
#
#
4. Hearing disorders in frequent neurodegnerative diseases in higher ages
4.1 Neurocognitive disorders
Neurocognitive disorders (NCD) are disorders that are associated with a subjective or objective loss of previously existing cognitive abilities in at least one of the 6 cognitive domains of complex attention, executive function, learning and memory, language, perceptual-motor, social cognition (cf. [Table 1)] and do not only occur exclusively in the context of delirium or can be explained by another existing mental disorder (such as major depression, schizophrenia) [3]. The DSM-5 distinguished between mild (minor) and severe (major) forms, which are seen on a continuum of cognitive and functional impairment. In minor NCD, moderate cognitive performance impairments are present but do not affect the ability to perform activities of daily living independently, although greater effort or compensatory strategies may be required. In major NCD cognitive performance has significantly declined and impairs independence in performing activities of daily living. The impairment in everyday activities can me mild (only instrumental activities such as household, handling money), moderate (limitations in basic activities of daily life like eating, dressing), or severe (complete dependence). The major NCD is intended to replace the widely used, and sometimes stigmatizing, term of dementia. Specific pathophysiological processes are known for the majority of neurocognitive disorders, allowing further specification of both minor and major NCD ([Table 2]).
Minor/major NCD due to |
---|
Alzheimer’s disease |
Fronto-temporal lobe degeneration |
Lewy body dementia |
Vascular disease |
Cranio-cerebral trauma |
Substance/drug consumption |
HIV infection |
Prion disease |
Parkinson’s syndrome |
Huntington disease |
Other medical factors |
Multiple etiologies |
Not specified |
4.1.1 Socio-economic relevance
Neurocognitive disorders predominantly affect older age, so a global increase in the number of cases is expected with demographic change. Based on data from the Global Burden of Disease Study of 2019 [115], the number of dementia patients worldwide was estimated to 55.4 million in 2019, and projections expect and increase to 152.8 million affected people in 2050 [116]. In some regions, however, decreases in new cases were observed: A recent analysis of the incidence rate over the last 25 years for Europe and North America showed a decrease in the incidence of dementia 13% per decade [252]. According to the German Alzheimer Society, approximately 1.8 million people in Germany were affected by dementia at the end of 2021, the vast majority (1.7 million) were over 65 years of age [117], and women were twice as likely to develop the disease than men. The number of newly diagnosed cases in the 65+ age group was estimated at 430,000 [117]. It is expected to increase to 2.8 million affected persons by 2050. At the same time, due to demographic change, the number of working-age individuals caring or paying for the care of dementia patients will decrease significantly [118].
In view of this major social challenge, prevention is of particular importance. An expert consortium recently identified 12 potentially modifiable risk factors, which together explain almost 40% of all dementias ([Table 3]). Hearing loss is the most important risk factor in middle age.
Time |
Risk factor |
Relative risk |
Attributable risk |
---|---|---|---|
Younger age (<45 years) |
Education |
1.6 |
7.1% |
Middle age (45-65 years) |
Hearing loss |
1.9 |
8.2% |
Cranio-cerebral trauma |
1.8 |
3.4% |
|
Hypertension |
1.6 |
1.9% |
|
Excessive alcohol consumption (>24g/d) |
1.2 |
0.8% |
|
Obesity (BMI ≥ 30) |
1.6 |
0.7% |
|
Higher age ( 65 Jahre) |
Smoking |
1.6 |
5.2% |
Depression |
1.9 |
3.9% |
|
Social isolation |
1.6 |
3.5% |
|
Physical inactivity |
1.4 |
1.6% |
|
Air pollution |
1.1 |
2.3% |
|
Diabetes |
1.5 |
1.1% |
*the attributable risk indicates the percentage by which one can reduce the incidence of disease if one completely eliminates the risk factor; BMI = Body-Mass-Index.
Societal changes such as improved education as well as adjustments in individual lifestyles could therefore contribute to a significant reduction in the risk of dementia and thus improve the quality of life in older age. For example, Norton et al. [119] estimated that even a prevalence reduction of 10-20% of each risk factor per decade could reduce the number of global Alzheimer patients by 8.8-16.2 million in 2050.
The national dementia strategy paper, adopted in 2020, seeks to address the increasing societal demands of dementia and aims to improve the lives and care of people with dementia in Germany. However, a concrete package of measures for the implementation of prevention strategies based on the above-mentioned risk factors is missing to date [120].
#
#
4.2 Alzheimer’s disease
Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is the most common cause of major NCD, accounting for an estimated 2/3 of all cases [121]. It is a progressive neurodegenerative disease with characteristic biological changes, primarily associated with memory impairment, leading to dementia [121]. The biological feature is the increasing deposit of β-amyloid and tau proteins in the brain of affected individuals. Approximately, 95% of the cases occur sporadically and usually after the age of 65 years (“late onset Alzheimer’s disease”, LOAD), in less than 5% of the cases, the first symptoms appear before the age of 60 years (“early onset Alzheimer’s disease”, EOAD) [122]. The sporadic form usually progresses slowly over years to decades, whereas more rapid courses are often observed in EOAD. The most important genetic risk factor for the sporadic disease is the so-called ApoE-4 allele of the gene for apolipoprotein E, which is involved in lipid metabolism and plays a role in amyloid deposit. For the early onset of the disease, 3 genes (presenilin-1, presenilin-2, amyloid precursor protein) have been identified so far as risk factors [121], which occur in a familial cluster in about 1% of all AD patients. In the course of the disease, β-amyloid accumulates between the nerve cells, initially in the form of oligomers, later as amyloid plaques, leading to a disturbance of nerve cell function and the associated development of clinical symptoms. Since about 20 years, subtypes of β-amyloid can be detected in CSF and used as biomarkers for AD (Aβ42 and Aβ42/Aβ40 ratio). In addition to extracellular amyloid deposits, intracellular deposits of defective tau proteins are typically found as neurofibrillary bundles or “tangles”. Total tau and phosphor-tau concentrations can be determined in the CSF. The first one indicates nonspecific nerve cell damage and may also be elevated in other neurodegenerative diseases or strokes. Phospho-tau (pTau), on the other hand, is significantly elevated exclusively in AD. The German S3 Dementia Guideline therefore recommends the combined measurement of Aβ42, total tau, and pTau to differentiate neurodegenerative and other causes in unclear dementias [123].
The amyloid deposits can also be visualized by positron emission tomography (amyloid PET).
The leading clinical symptom is slowly progressive disturbances primarily of learning and memory, but also of attention as well as spatial and temporal orientation [121] [122]. Radiologically, in addition to a general brain volume reduction, atrophy of the medial temporal lobe, especially the hippocampus, is typically found [124]. In approximately 10% of the cases, the disease manifests with atypical symptoms such as loss of visuospatial abilities (posterior parietal atrophy, Benson syndrome) [125] or as frontal or logopenic variants [126] [127], both of which resemble typical fronto-temporal dementias. Parieto-temporal metabolic disorders can be visualized by fluorodeoxyglucose PET (FDG-PET) and assist in confirming the diagnosis. Cognitive function loss is usually accompanied by neuropsychiatric symptoms, such as apathy, agitation, anxiety, sleep disturbances, and depression.
Alzheimer’s disease is nowadays understood as a continuum, as the biological processes begin years to decades before the onset of the first symptoms and result in cognitive changes as the disease progresses. Based on the biological markers, it is possible to identify patients as affected by Alzheimer’s disease already at the preclinical stage or at the stage of mild cognitive impairment (minor NCD or mild cognitive impairment, MCI).
4.2.1 Hearing loss and Alzheimer’s disease
Already in 1993, Sinha et al. [128] reported the involvement of the auditory system in Alzheimer’s disease. Amyloid plaques and intracellular neurofibrils were detected in the medial geniculate corpus and inferior colliculus, primary auditory cortex, and auditory association areas. A functional feature of temporo-parietal changes in AD is considered to be a disturbance in auditory scene analysis, i. e., the ability to identify auditory objects – e. g., a speaker – and to follow them even in the presence of noise [129] [130] [131] [132] [133]. For example, Goll et al. [129] demonstrated that Alzheimer patients were significantly worse at discriminating spectrally and temporally altered environmental sounds compared to healthy individuals with comparable peripheral auditory thresholds when non-verbal working memory was taken into account, while the ability to perceive pitch and timbre remained the same. Coeberg et al. [134] also found significantly more auditory agnosia for environmental sounds in patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease compared to healthy individuals, with 37% of patients showing impairment in recognition and 57% in naming test sounds. The mean hearing threshold of the patients affected by agnosia was significantly higher, independent of age. This means, peripheral hearing loss in combination with Alzheimer’s pathology increases the likelihood of the occurrence of further central auditory deficits (in this study, an odds ratio of 13.75 versus healthy subjects).
Already in 1986, Uhlmann et al. [135] described a correlation between peripheral hearing and significantly faster cognitive performance loss in AD. In a long-term study of 639 cognitively healthy individuals at study inclusion [136], an increase in dementia risk of 20% was shown for each 10 dB increase in mean hearing threshold. Broken down by degree of hearing loss, the hazard ratios were 1.89 for low, 3.00 for moderate, and 4.94 for severe hearing loss. A meta-analysis of 33 studies confirmed the association of peripheral hearing and cognitive function [137]. The cognitive performance of patients with hearing loss was lower than that of hearing healthy individuals, regardless of whether the hearing loss was treated or not. Nevertheless, the difference between individuals with treated hearing loss and hearing healthy individuals was more than half. Hearing loss had a negative effect on all cognitive domains investigated (attention, processing speed, working memory, long-term memory, executive functions, semantic and lexical knowledge), but the effect size was small (accounting for 4-6% of variance).
A similar relationship has been shown for central hearing impairment. As early as 1996, Gates et al. [138] reported a 6-fold higher risk of dementia for patients with central hearing impairment, and further large longitudinal and cross-sectional studies came to similar conclusions [139] [140] [141] [142] [143]. Central hearing impairment in particular has therefore been discussed as a possible harbinger of later dementia [133] [138] [140] [144]. A recent meta-analysis [145] concluded that although a number of subjective audiometric methods for assessing central auditory processing (including speech in noise, dichotic hearing/binaural processing, time-compressed speech) can discriminate well between normal cognitive aging and mild cognitive impairment or AD, a reliable differentiation between MCI and AD has not yet been possible. Moreover, whether in the preclinical phase of AD without cognitive impairment these investigations can contribute to an earlier diagnosis than by the currently known neurological and biological markers remains open [146].
Auditory, event-related potentials could potentially close this gap. In a study of 26 patients with a positive family history of AD, it was shown that carriers of mutations in the presenilin-1 and APP genes already show significant changes in central auditory potentials even before cognitive deficits become clinically manifest [147]. The latency delay of late auditory-evoked potentials N100, P200, N200, and P300 demonstrated in this study was taken as an electrophysiological sign of slower cortical information processing. A later meta-analysis by Morrison et al. [92], evaluating studies published between 2005 and 2017 on auditory-evoked potentials in patients over 60 years of age, concluded that P300 and N200 are appropriate electrophysiological markers for distinguishing normal cognitive aging, mild cognitive impairment, and AD.
#
#
4.3 Parkinson’s syndrome (PS)
Parkinson’s syndrome is the most common neurodegenerative disease after Alzheimer’s disease [148] [149]. According to a recent epidemiological estimate based on health insurance data of 3.7 million insured persons, approximately 420,000 people in Germany were affected in 2015 [150], the standardized prevalence amounted to 511.4/100,000.
The incidence increases with higher age: while about 50/100,000 of the 65-year-old people are affected, about 400/100,000 patients are found in the age group of 85 years and older [151]. Due to demographic change, but also earlier detection, the number of people affected by PS in the EU is expected to increase to about 4.25 million by 2050 [152]. Parkinson’s syndrome (PS) comprises an etiologically and phenotypically heterogeneous group of disorders. In addition to idiopathic Parkinson’s syndrome (IPS, about 75% of all cases), a distinction is made between genetic forms as well as Parkinson’s syndromes in the context of other neurodegenerative diseases (atypical PS, multisystem atrophy, Lewy body-type dementia, progressive supranuclear gaze palsy, corticobasal degeneration) and symptomatic (secondary) Parkinson’s syndrome (drug-induced, posttraumatic, toxic, metabolic, inflammatory, tumor-related) [153] [154] [155] [156]. In addition to the cardinal motor symptoms (akinesia/bradykinesia, resting tremor, rigor, and postural instability), a wide variety of accompanying sensory, autonomic, psychological, and cognitive symptoms may occur and significantly impair quality of life [157] [158]. Cognitive disorders mainly affect executive functions, such as planning, anticipatory thinking, working memory, and difficulties in switching attention between different tasks.
The incidence of so-called Parkinson’s dementia is estimated in international cross-sectional studies to be between 20-44%, which corresponds to a 3-6-fold higher risk of disease for Parkinson patients compared to non-affected individuals [159] [160]. In a German cross-sectional study of 873 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s syndrome, 28.6% of the patients met the diagnostic criteria for dementia according to DSM-5, with the frequency increasing significantly with higher age as well as disease stage [158]. The British CamPalGN study followed 142 patients newly diagnosed with IPS between 2000 and 2002 [161], 46% of this population developed dementia within the 10-year follow-up period, again including age at diagnosis and disease stage as significant prognostic factors.
4.3.1 Hearing loss and Parkinson’s syndrome
Hearing loss is discussed as another non-motor accompanying symptom of PS [162] [163] [164] [165] [166]. Several studies have shown that hearing impaired people suffer more frequently from PS [162] [167]. In pure-tone audiometry, predominantly high-frequency losses [168] [169] [170] [171] are found that exceed the extent of merely presbycusis [169] [172] [173] [174] [175]. A British case-control study of 55 patients with PS and early onset (≤55 years) found unilateral or bilateral hearing thresholds deviating from the age norm in 64.7% of patients and 28% of the age- and sex-matched control group [169]. No difference was found in brainstem audiometry between the two groups in this study, so the authors assumed pure cochlear involvement. The suggestion of dopamine-dependent cochlear dysfunction is supported by evidence of reduced DPOAE amplitudes that improved with levodopa substitution [172]; in this study, DPOAE dysfunction correlated with the clinical severity of Parkinson’s syndrome. Another study group found additional significant lateral differences. Cochlear function measured by DPOAE and pure-tone audiometry was not only worse in Parkinson patients than in the control group of the same age, but also significantly more pronounced on the ipsilateral ear of motor symptoms [176].
Beyond tone audiometric changes, difficulties in the perception of rhythms and tonal differences [177] [178] have been reported.
A number of studies have demonstrated changes in the morphology, latency, and interpeak intervals of early auditory brainstem response (ABR) in PS patients [168] [179] [180]. Similarly, reduced amplitudes and prolonged latencies of vestibular evoked potentials (VEMP) were found [179] [181] [182]. The event-related potential P3 is suitable to detect stage and progression of Parkinson’s syndrome. The subject is offered sequences of repetitive standard stimuli that are rarely interrupted by a deviant stimulus (so-called oddball paradigm). The evoked potential (P300, P3a, P3b) is dependent on attention and working memory and therefore seems to be suitable to assess the impairment of executive functions in PS [183] [184] [185] [186] [187]. With increasing severity, there is a reduction in amplitude as well as prolongation of latency, so that patients with and without Parkinson’s dementia can be distinguished electrophysiologically [188] [189].
Although auditory stimuli and music are used for the treatment of Parkinson-related gait disorders and postural instability [190] [191] [192], the importance of auditory rehabilitation for Parkinson patients is not discussed in therapy studies.
#
#
#
5. Correlation of hearing loss and cognitive impairment
The importance of cognitive processes for speech comprehension, especially in challenging listening situations, is well established. Age-related deficits lead to restrictions in communication ability, social isolation and, associated with this, to psychological stress and reduced quality of life. The question of a possible causal relationship between hearing loss and reduced cognitive abilities up to manifest dementia has increasingly become the focus of scientific research in recent years (see comprehensive reviews in e. g. [53] [146] [166] [193] [194] [195] [196]). The analysis of already published study results is complicated by the great heterogeneity of the collected data, both in terms of audiological and cognitive parameters, as well as in terms of the studied groups, recorded influencing factors, and duration of follow-up.
Usually, the pure-tone hearing threshold is used for the assessment of (peripheral) hearing loss, but already here, there are differences in the grouping of the included subjects, depending on the method used to differentiate between subjects with and without hearing loss.
On the basis of 3 long-term studies [136] [197] [198] (at least 5 years of follow-up) of subjects without cognitive impairment with tone audiometrically determined hearing threshold, the Lancet Commission [24] [25] calculated a relative risk of 1.9 for developing dementia in the presence of hearing impairment (defined as hearing loss greater than 25 dB HL in the pure-tone audiogram) in middle age (55 years and older) compared with normal hearing subjects. Hearing loss in middle age has been identified as the most important modifiable risk factor for developing dementia.
Few studies explicitly address the relationship between central auditory disorders and dementia or cognitive deficits in old age. A meta-analysis by Dryden et al. [199] identified 25 studies that investigated the relationship between cognitive performance and speech understanding in noise as a measure of central hearing impairment. For both the subset of studies that included only peripherally normal hearing subjects (16 articles) and studies that also included subjects with at most moderate hearing loss (up to 70 dB HL, 9 studies), the overall correlation (r=0.31 [normal hearing], r=0.32 [up to moderate hearing loss]) of cognitive function and speech understanding in noise was weak. Broken down by cognitive domains, the strongest correlation was seen for processing speed (r=0.39), followed by inhibition control (r=0.34), working memory (r=0.28), and episodic memory (r=0.26), whereas global measures of crystalline intelligence showed a significantly weaker correlation (r=0.18).
Wayne and Johnsrude [194] state that the use of global cognitive screening tests such as the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCa [200]), the Mini-Mental State Test (MMST [201]), and the Modified Mini-Mental State Test (3MS [202]) in normal aging individuals shows little variability, and thus may underestimate the impact of hearing loss on cognitive function.
At the same time, the presence of hearing impairment may interfere with the performance in cognitive tests and lead to an overestimation of the cognitive deficit present, especially when instructions are given verbally, as shown by several studies in normal-hearing, cognitively healthy subjects with simulated hearing loss [203] [204] [205]. Therefore, special versions of cognitive screening instruments for hearing-impaired people have been developed, which should be used preferentially in the future (refer to Völter et al. [206] for a comprehensive overview).
5.1 Explanatory models for the interaction of hearing and cognition
In order to explain the relationship between (age-related) hearing loss and cognitive decline, a number of models are discussed, which will be briefly described below. A comprehensive review is provided by Wayne and Johnsrude [194].
5.1.1 Model 1: Cognitive load on perception hypothesis
Declining cognitive capacity places increasing load on perception so that no longer sufficient resources are available for the processing of sensory information. This leads to an audiometrically measurable hearing impairment [207] [208]. A study by Kiely et al. [209] seems to confirm this theory. After analyzing longitudinal data from a total of 4221 subjects, the authors concluded that, in addition to age and hypertension, a score of less than 24 on the Mini-Mental State Test was among the independent predictors of annual hearing threshold deterioration. Ex post, it remains unclear to what extent the hearing impairment itself affected the test result, because the test used was presented verbally ([Fig 4a]).
#
5.1.2. Model 2: Information degradation hypothesis
This model assumes that reduced or impaired peripheral hearing triggers an upward cascade in which cognitive resources are applied to compensate for the hearing impairment, rendering them unavailable for other cognitive processes [207] [210]. Evidence for this assumption is high; for example, several studies have shown that the ability to recall words or sentences deteriorates during a demanding perceptual experiment in elderly subjects [17] [211]. The associated increased listening effort has negative effects on working memory and inhibition control [17]. The cognitive loss in this model is reversible – it is assumed that if peripheral input is improved, e. g., by compensating for hearing loss with hearing aids, at least partial recovery of cognitive performance is possible ([Fig 4b]).
#
5.1.3 Model 3: Sensory deprivation hypothesis
This model assumes that a lasting shift in resources to compensate for perceptual deficits leads to a permanent loss of cognitive function. Neuroplastic remodeling in central auditory areas and neurovascular and neurophysiological changes similar to those seen in dementia are postulated as possible mechanisms [106] [212] [213] [214]. For congenital or early acquired hearing loss, the associated neuroplastic changes are already well established [215] [216], but cognitive performance is little affected [217]. Sensory deprivation alone is thus insufficient as an explanatory model for cognitive loss in old age ([Fig 4c]).
#
5.1.4 Model 4: Common cause hypothesis
General age-related neurodegeneration processes could have negative consequences for both cognitive performance and sensory perception [207] [208]. For example, the decrease in processing speed is discussed as one such common factor [218]. In addition to genetic causes [219], cerebrovascular disease [220] and general loss of physical functioning have been considered as possible mechanisms ([Fig 4d]).
#
5.1.5 Multifactorial model
None of the above assumptions alone can explain all observed changes in older age; a combination of several effects is most likely. Wayne and Johnsrude [194] therefore postulated a multifactorial model illustrating the interdependence of sensory and cognitive processes ([Fig 5]).
Age-related neurodegenerative changes increase cognitive demands and, in combination with sensory deficits, lead to impaired perception. Compensation for perceptual deficits increases cognitive load, which can lead to declines in mental performance. Other sensory deficits (e. g., impaired vision or balance) amplify the impairment. The communication disorder caused by the hearing loss promotes social isolation and loneliness and with it depression and frailty – the latter being further risk factors for cognitive decline independent of hearing loss [53] [221].
#
#
#
6. Can treatment of hearing loss reduce cognitive impairment?
Due to the widespread availability of hearing aids, treatment of age-related hearing loss is perceived as an achievable target for dementia prevention. However, testing the effectiveness of such an intervention presents unique challenges. For example, in the context of an observational study, it is difficult to monitor the quality of hearing aid fitting as well as the duration of daily use. The latter is now facilitated by the possibility of data logging by the hearing aid. A recent study on datasets of more than 15,000 hearing aid users was able to objectify the considerable inter- but also intraindividual variance in daily hearing aid use [222]. At the same time, factors such as socio-economic status, education level, social environment, communication behavior, and access to health care play a role in both hearing aid use and risk of cognitive decline, making independent assessment of the impact of hearing rehabilitation difficult. Large epidemiologic aging studies in the past have partially included hearing threshold but not systematic hearing aid use (e. g., for the German-speaking countries [223]).
A multicenter, randomized-controlled longitudinal intervention study initiated in 2018 in the USA including more than 800 70-84-year-old individuals without dementia with low to moderate hearing loss comparing the efficacy of hearing aid provision with health education alone with parallel collection of audiologic data as well as cognitive performance over a 3-year period (ACHIEVE study, [224]) intends to address the issue, but completion is not expected until late 2022 at the earliest.
Regarding the different intervention options, currently most data are found on conventional hearing aid fitting, in recent years increasingly also on cochlear implantation.
6.1 Provision of hearing aids
The Lancet Commission [24] cites 3 recent studies to support the possible preventive effect of hearing aid use. A prospective study demonstrated a correlation between increased incidence of dementia in subjects with self-reported hearing loss within the 25-year observation period only if they did not use hearing aids [225]. The cross-sectional study of Ray et al. [226] also found cognitive deficits only in the subgroup of hearing impaired subjects who did not use their hearing aids, but the groups studied varied considerably in age and severity of hearing loss. The long-term study by Maharani et al. [227] found a slowing of age-related functional loss in episodic memory after the onset of hearing aid use.
In a comprehensive systematic analysis of long-term studies published between 1990 and 2020 on the relationship between hearing aid use and cognitive function [228], the authors concluded that to date, based on the current body of studies, no definitive conclusion on the preventive effect of hearing aid use can be drawn. The methodology of the existing studies is extremely heterogeneous, of particular importance is the generally short follow-up period with regard to the rather slow age-related cognitive function loss. In addition to the aforementioned study by Maharani et al. [227], the authors were able to identify only 1 other study in which subjects were followed-up for at least 10 years, which did not reveal any differences between intervention group (with hearing aids) and control group for any cognitive measures [229]. In addition, a common problem in comparative studies was large hearing threshold differences between intervention and control groups. Furthermore, hearing aid compliance was poorly reported or not reported at all in 9/17 studies, leaving it unclear to what extent subjects used the hearing aid adequately. The greatest potential benefit of hearing aid provision appeared to be in the area of executive function – after all, 6/11 studies found improvement [228]. Two out of 4 studies found significant improvement with hearing aid use on screening tests (MMST). However, it was not reported whether the hearing impaired version was used, so it cannot be excluded that due to hearing impairment in baseline testing, cognitive function loss was overestimated and the improvement found by using the hearing aids was only due to a better understanding of the verbally presented tasks.
#
6.2 Cochlear implantation
It is well established that elderly patients with severe hearing loss or deafness benefit from cochlear implantation in terms of speech understanding and quality of life (e. g., [230] [231] [232] [233] [234]). Compared to normal-hearing individuals, the incoming signal is already highly degraded by the signal processing of the cochlear implant, which requires a greater input of cognitive resources to understand speech in the first place. Assuming that aging processes of the central auditory pathway affect CI recipients to the same extent as normal-hearing individuals, older CI users are at an even greater disadvantage because impaired temporal processing further deteriorates the already degraded signal [235]. As in normal-hearing individuals, working memory function affects speech comprehension [236] [237], and linguistic context can be used to some extent to improve speech comprehension [238].
In recent years, a number of studies have been published explicitly addressing the alteration of (global) cognitive functions by cochlear implantation [239] [240] [241] [242] [243] [244] [245] [246] [247] [248] [249] [250] [251]. Similar to studies on hearing aid users, the neurocognitive test batteries chosen varied widely, although tests suitable for hearing impaired people were increasingly used [244] [245] [246] [247] [248] [251] [252]. The follow-up period was relatively short (12 months) in most studies, probably because the long-term studies in questions were initiated only in recent years. Four research groups reported results after 18 [251], 24 [246], at least 25 [242], and 60 months [240]. Positive effects, especially on executive functions, were already reported within the short follow-up period. A limiting factor is the small number of cases – mostly<20 patients have been included [241] [242] [243] [244] [252]. The largest number of participants with simultaneous use of a neurocognitive test battery adapted for hearing impaired subjects has been studied so far by Völter et al. [246] [247] [248]. During a follow-up period of at least 24 months, 71 elderly CI patients (mean age at implantation 66.03 years) showed significant improvements in executive functions (attention, working memory, inhibition) already after 6 months compared to preoperative performance, and after 12 months, memory and word fluency had also significantly improved. After 24 months, there was an improvement in processing speed; inhibition control (flanker) was no longer significantly better, and there were no changes in mental flexibility throughout the study period. Preoperatively, the performance of 12 of the 71 subjects was below 68% confidence interval in 3 or more subtests; after 12 months, this was the case in only 3/71 subjects. By the end of the study, 5/71 subjects had deteriorated in more than 2 subtests. Cognitive performance had no significant effect on speech comprehension at rest.
A similar result was already reported by Mosnier et al. [239] in their investigation of 94 CI users aged 65-85 years: Of 37 subjects with preoperatively worse cognitive function, 81% improved within the first 12 months, and performance remained stable in 19%. Regarding dementia development, the follow-up study by the same research group is particularly interesting [240]. 80 subjects of the original 94 included were still alive 5 years after implantation, 70 of whom could be followed-up. Before cochlear implantation, 31 subjects had cognitive performance in the range of mild cognitive impairment. Of these, 32% recovered to normal function, 6% developed dementia, and 61% remained stable. Of the 38 subjects with preoperatively normal function, none developed dementia during the follow-up period, but in 32% of the cases, cognitive performance was in the range of mild cognitive impairment after 5 years. A correlation with the achieved speech comprehension could not be proven.
Overall, all studies published so far show a clear positive, at least stabilizing, mostly even improving effect of cochlear implantation.
#
#
7. Outlook
Sensory and cognitive deficits are closely linked via complex bottom-up and top-down processes. The consequences of both normal and pathological aging processes will inevitably pose major challenges to our society in the future. The realization that a number of risk factors can be modified already in young and middle ages offers opportunities for prevention. In particular, the consistent treatment of hearing loss must become an even greater focus of health education, also in view of the threat of social isolation and depression as further risk factors for cognitive decline, in order to increase the alarmingly low rate of care, even in industrialized countries. It is essential to take into account the special needs of the elderly, both with regard to the operation of hearing systems (fine motor requirements when changing batteries vs. using rechargeable batteries, simple operating structure/coupling with external systems) and the fitting process (possibly longer habituation phase, slower processing speed, lower differentiation acuity when comparing different settings). Appropriate compensation for the increased time required for consultation and repeated adjustment would increase the incentive for providers to devote the necessary attention to this patient group. The higher costs of care would be offset by a significantly improved quality of life and longer cognitive function preservation in the case of successful adaptation, which could lead to a reduction in the costs of care and thus to a reduction in the burden on society as a whole. To validate the success of the fitting, further long-term studies are required that record in detail both cognitive function and hearing performance as well as the type and extent of use of hearing systems and apply measurement methods that are methodologically adapted to possible cognitive and sensory deficits.
#
#
-
Literatur
- 1 Stangl, Werner. Online Lexikon für Psychologie und Pädagogik
- 2 Flanagan DP, Dixon SG. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll Theory of Cognitive Abilities. In: Encyclopedia of Special Education. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2014
- 3 American Psychiatric Association, Peter Falkai, Hans-Ulrich Wittchen Diagnostisches und Statistisches Manual Psychischer Störungen DSM-5. 2. korrigierte Auflage 2018. Hogrefe; 2018
- 4 Tucker-Drob EM. Neurocognitive functions and everyday functions change together in old age. Neuropsychology 2011; 25: 368-377 DOI: 10.1037/a0022348.
- 5 Tucker-Drob EM. Cognitive Aging and Dementia: A Life Span Perspective. Annu Rev. Dev Psychol 2019; 1: 177-196 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-085204.
- 6 Baltes PB. [Age and aging as incomplete architecture of human ontogenesis]. Z Gerontol Geriatr 1999; 32: 433-448 DOI: 10.1007/s003910050141.
- 7 Tucker-Drob EM, de la Fuente J, Köhncke Y. et al. A strong dependency between changes in fluid and crystallized abilities in human cognitive aging. Sci Adv 2022; 8: eabj2422 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abj2422.
- 8 Hartshorne JK, Germine LT. When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychol Sci 2015; 26: 433-443 DOI: 10.1177/0956797614567339.
- 9 Tucker-Drob EM. Global and domain-specific changes in cognition throughout adulthood. Dev Psychol 2011; 47: 331-343 DOI: 10.1037/a0021361.
- 10 Buckner RL. Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 2004; 44: 195-208 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006.
- 11 Hedden T, Gabrieli JDE. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004; 5: 87-96 DOI: 10.1038/nrn1323.
- 12 Jagust W. Vulnerable neural systems and the borderland of brain aging and neurodegeneration. Neuron 2013; 77: 219-234 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.002.
- 13 Baltes PB, Dittmann-Kohli F, Kliegl R. Reserve capacity of the elderly in aging-sensitive tests of fluid intelligence: replication and extension. Psychol Aging 1986; 1: 172-177 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.1.2.172.
- 14 Stern Y, Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Bartrés-Faz D. et al. Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement J Alzheimers Assoc 2020; 16: 1305-1311 DOI: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219.
- 15 Tucker AM, Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in aging. Curr Alzheimer Res 2011; 8: 354-360 DOI: 10.2174/156720511795745320.
- 16 Stenfelt S, Rönnberg J. The signal-cognition interface: interactions between degraded auditory signals and cognitive processes. Scand J Psychol 2009; 50: 385-393 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00748.x.
- 17 Wingfield A, Tun PA. Cognitive Supports and Cognitive Constraints on Comprehension of Spoken Language. J Am Acad Audiol 2007; 18: 548-558 DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.18.7.3.
- 18 Gordon-Salant S, Shader MJ, Wingfield A. Age-Related Changes in Speech Understanding: Peripheral Versus Cognitive Influences. In: Helfer KS, Bartlett EL, Popper AN, et al., Hrsg. Aging and Hearing: Causes and Consequences. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2020: 199-230
- 19 Johnson JCS, Marshall CR, Weil RS. et al. Hearing and dementia: from ears to brain. Brain J Neurol 2021; 144: 391-401 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awaa429.
- 20 World Health Organization World report on hearing. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2021
- 21 Davis A, McMahon CM, Pichora-Fuller KM. et al. Aging and Hearing Health: The Life-course Approach. The Gerontologist 2016; 56: S256-S267 DOI: 10.1093/geront/gnw033.
- 22 Lin FR, Yaffe K, Xia J. et al. Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern Med 2013; 173: 293-299 DOI: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.1868.
- 23 Loughrey DG, Kelly ME, Kelley GA. et al. Association of Age-Related Hearing Loss With Cognitive Function, Cognitive Impairment, and Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Otolaryngol-- Head Neck Surg 2018; 144: 115-126 DOI: 10.1001/jamaoto.2017.2513.
- 24 Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A. et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet Lond Engl 2020; 396: 413-446 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6.
- 25 Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V. et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet Lond Engl 2017; 390: 2673-2734 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6.
- 26 Rutherford BR, Brewster K, Golub JS. et al. Sensation and Psychiatry: Linking Age-Related Hearing Loss to Late-Life Depression and Cognitive Decline. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175: 215-224 DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17040423.
- 27 Brewster K, Choi CJ, He X. et al. Hearing Rehabilitative Treatment for Older Adults With Comorbid Hearing Loss and Depression: Effects on Depressive Symptoms and Executive Function. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry Off J Am Assoc Geriatr Psychiatry 2022; 30: 448-458 DOI: 10.1016/j.jagp.2021.08.006.
- 28 Brewster KK, Pavlicova M, Stein A. et al. A pilot randomized controlled trial of hearing aids to improve mood and cognition in older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2020; 35: 842-850 DOI: 10.1002/gps.5311.
- 29 Bigelow RT, Reed NS, Brewster KK. et al. Association of Hearing Loss With Psychological Distress and Utilization of Mental Health Services Among Adults in the United States. JAMA Netw Open 2020; 3: e2010986 DOI: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.10986.
- 30 Orji A, Kamenov K, Dirac M. et al. Global and regional needs, unmet needs and access to hearing aids. Int J Audiol 2020; 59: 166-172 DOI: 10.1080/14992027.2020.1721577.
- 31 Liberman MC, Kujawa SG. Cochlear synaptopathy in acquired sensorineural hearing loss: Manifestations and mechanisms. Hear Res 2017; 349: 138-147 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.01.003.
- 32 Keithley EM. Pathology and mechanisms of cochlear aging. J Neurosci Res 2020; 98: 1674-1684 DOI: 10.1002/jnr.24439.
- 33 Frisina RD, Ding B, Zhu X. et al. Age-related hearing loss: prevention of threshold declines, cell loss and apoptosis in spiral ganglion neurons. Aging 2016; 8: 2081-2099 DOI: 10.18632/aging.101045.
- 34 Kujawa SG, Liberman MC. Synaptopathy in the noise-exposed and aging cochlea: Primary neural degeneration in acquired sensorineural hearing loss. Hear Res 2015; 330: 191-199 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.02.009.
- 35 Wu PZ, Liberman LD, Bennett K. et al. Primary Neural Degeneration in the Human Cochlea: Evidence for Hidden Hearing Loss in the Aging Ear. Neuroscience 2019; 407: 8-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.07.053.
- 36 Gates GA, Mills JH. Presbycusis. The Lancet 2005; 366: 1111-1120 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67423-5.
- 37 Dubno JR, Eckert MA, Lee F-S. et al. Classifying human audiometric phenotypes of age-related hearing loss from animal models. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol JARO 2013; 14: 687-701 DOI: 10.1007/s10162-013-0396-x.
- 38 Fischer N, Weber B, Riechelmann H. [Presbycusis – Age Related Hearing Loss]. Laryngorhinootologie 2016; 95: 497-510 DOI: 10.1055/s-0042-106918.
- 39 Michel O. [DIN EN ISO 7029:2017-06 : The current DIN thresholds for evaluating normal hearing]. HNO 2021; 69: 1014-1018 DOI: 10.1007/s00106-021-01111-3.
- 40 Tremblay KL, Pinto A, Fischer ME. et al. Self-Reported Hearing Difficulties Among Adults With Normal Audiograms: The Beaver Dam Offspring Study. Ear Hear 2015; 36: e290-e299 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000195.
- 41 Schaette R, McAlpine D. Tinnitus with a normal audiogram: physiological evidence for hidden hearing loss and computational model. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 2011; 31: 13452-13457 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2156-11.2011.
- 42 Bajin MD, Dahm V, Lin VYW. Hidden hearing loss: current concepts. Curr Opin Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2022; DOI: 10.1097/MOO.0000000000000824.
- 43 C Kohrman D, Wan G, Cassinotti L et al. Hidden Hearing Loss: A Disorder with Multiple Etiologies and Mechanisms. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med 2020; 10: a035493 DOI: 10.1101/cshperspect.a035493.
- 44 Pienkowski M. On the Etiology of Listening Difficulties in Noise Despite Clinically Normal Audiograms. Ear Hear 2017; 38: 135-148 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000388.
- 45 Plack CJ, Barker D, Prendergast G. Perceptual consequences of „hidden“ hearing loss. Trends Hear 2014; 18: 2331216514550621 DOI: 10.1177/2331216514550621.
- 46 Parthasarathy A, Kujawa SG. Synaptopathy in the Aging Cochlea: Characterizing Early-Neural Deficits in Auditory Temporal Envelope Processing. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 2018; 38: 7108-7119 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3240-17.2018.
- 47 Wan G, Corfas G. Transient auditory nerve demyelination as a new mechanism for hidden hearing loss. Nat Commun 2017; 8: 14487 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14487.
- 48 Choi JE, Seok JM, Ahn J. et al. Hidden hearing loss in patients with Charcot-Marie-Tooth disease type 1A. Sci Rep 2018; 8: 10335 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-28501-y.
- 49 Mulders WHAM, Chin IL, Robertson D. Persistent hair cell malfunction contributes to hidden hearing loss. Hear Res 2018; 361: 45-51 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2018.02.001.
- 50 Hoben R, Easow G, Pevzner S. et al. Outer Hair Cell and Auditory Nerve Function in Speech Recognition in Quiet and in Background Noise. Front Neurosci 2017; 11: 157 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00157.
- 51 Sergeyenko Y, Lall K, Liberman MC. et al. Age-related cochlear synaptopathy: an early-onset contributor to auditory functional decline. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 2013; 33: 13686-13694 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1783-13.2013.
- 52 Grant KJ, Mepani AM, Wu P. et al. Electrophysiological markers of cochlear function correlate with hearing-in-noise performance among audiometrically normal subjects. J Neurophysiol 2020; 124: 418-431 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00016.2020.
- 53 Jayakody DMP, Friedland PL, Martins RN. et al. Impact of Aging on the Auditory System and Related Cognitive Functions: A Narrative Review. Front Neurosci 2018; 12: 125 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00125.
- 54 Ouda L, Profant O, Syka J. Age-related changes in the central auditory system. Cell Tissue Res 2015; 361: 337-358 DOI: 10.1007/s00441-014-2107-2.
- 55 Hedman AM, van Haren NEM, Schnack HG. et al. Human brain changes across the life span: a review of 56 longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 2012; 33: 1987-2002 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21334.
- 56 Mori S, Onda K, Fujita S. et al. Brain atrophy in middle age using magnetic resonance imaging scans from Japan’s health screening programme. Brain Commun 2022; 4: fcac211 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac211.
- 57 Miller KL, Alfaro-Almagro F, Bangerter NK. et al. Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat Neurosci 2016; 19: 1523-1536 DOI: 10.1038/nn.4393.
- 58 Lemaitre H, Goldman AL, Sambataro F. et al. Normal age-related brain morphometric changes: nonuniformity across cortical thickness, surface area and gray matter volume?. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33: e1-e9 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.013.
- 59 Raz N, Gunning FM, Head D. et al. Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cereb Cortex N Y N 1991 1997; 7: 268-282 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/7.3.268.
- 60 Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Head D. et al. Differential aging of the medial temporal lobe: a study of a five-year change. Neurology 2004; 62: 433-438 DOI: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000106466.09835.46.
- 61 Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM. et al. Vascular health and longitudinal changes in brain and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. Neuropsychology 2007; 21: 149-157 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.149.
- 62 Westlye LT, Walhovd KB, Dale AM. et al. Life-span changes of the human brain white matter: diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and volumetry. Cereb Cortex N Y N 1991 2010; 20: 2055-2068 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhp280.
- 63 Vidal-Pineiro D, Parker N, Shin J. et al. Cellular correlates of cortical thinning throughout the lifespan. Sci Rep 2020; 10: 21803 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-78471-3.
- 64 Scahill RI, Frost C, Jenkins R. et al. A longitudinal study of brain volume changes in normal aging using serial registered magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Neurol 2003; 60: 989-994 DOI: 10.1001/archneur.60.7.989.
- 65 Braak H, Thal DR, Ghebremedhin E. et al. Stages of the pathologic process in Alzheimer disease: age categories from 1 to 100 years. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2011; 70: 960-969 DOI: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e318232a379.
- 66 Pettemeridou E, Kallousia E, Constantinidou F. Regional Brain Volume, Brain Reserve and MMSE Performance in Healthy Aging From the NEUROAGE Cohort: Contributions of Sex, Education, and Depression Symptoms. Front Aging Neurosci 2021; 13: 711301 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.711301.
- 67 Kalpouzos G, Persson J, Nyberg L. Local brain atrophy accounts for functional activity differences in normal aging. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33: 623.e1-623.e13 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.02.021.
- 68 Lin FR, Ferrucci L, An Y. et al. Association of hearing impairment with brain volume changes in older adults. NeuroImage 2014; 90: 84-92 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.059.
- 69 Husain FT, Medina RE, Davis CW. et al. Neuroanatomical changes due to hearing loss and chronic tinnitus: a combined VBM and DTI study. Brain Res 2011; 1369: 74-88 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.095.
- 70 Boyen K, Langers DRM, de Kleine E. et al. Gray matter in the brain: differences associated with tinnitus and hearing loss. Hear Res 2013; 295: 67-78 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.02.010.
- 71 Rosemann S, Thiel CM. Neuroanatomical changes associated with age-related hearing loss and listening effort. Brain Struct Funct 2020; 225: 2689-2700 DOI: 10.1007/s00429-020-02148-w.
- 72 Peelle JE, Troiani V, Grossman M. et al. Hearing loss in older adults affects neural systems supporting speech comprehension. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 2011; 31: 12638-12643 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2559-11.2011.
- 73 Eckert MA, Cute SL, Vaden KI. et al. Auditory cortex signs of age-related hearing loss. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol JARO 2012; 13: 703-713 DOI: 10.1007/s10162-012-0332-5.
- 74 Chang Y, Lee S-H, Lee Y-J. et al. Auditory neural pathway evaluation on sensorineural hearing loss using diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroReport 2004; 15: 1699-1703 DOI: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000134584.10207.1a.
- 75 Profant O, Balogová Z, Dezortová M. et al. Metabolic changes in the auditory cortex in presbycusis demonstrated by MR spectroscopy. Exp Gerontol 2013; 48: 795-800 DOI: 10.1016/j.exger.2013.04.012.
- 76 Gao F, Wang G, Ma W. et al. Decreased auditory GABA+concentrations in presbycusis demonstrated by edited magnetic resonance spectroscopy. NeuroImage 2015; 106: 311-316 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.023.
- 77 Peelle JE, Wingfield A. The Neural Consequences of Age-Related Hearing Loss. Trends Neurosci 2016; 39: 486-497 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.05.001.
- 78 Gordon-Salant S, Yeni-Komshian G, Fitzgibbons P. The role of temporal cues in word identification by younger and older adults: effects of sentence context. J Acoust Soc Am 2008; 124: 3249-3260 DOI: 10.1121/1.2982409.
- 79 Schvartz KC, Chatterjee M, Gordon-Salant S. Recognition of spectrally degraded phonemes by younger, middle-aged, and older normal-hearing listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 2008; 124: 3972-3988 DOI: 10.1121/1.2997434.
- 80 Goupell MJ, Gaskins CR, Shader MJ. et al. Age-Related Differences in the Processing of Temporal Envelope and Spectral Cues in a Speech Segment. Ear Hear 2017; 38: e335-e342 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000447.
- 81 Gordon-Salant S, Yeni-Komshian GH, Fitzgibbons PJ. Recognition of accented English in quiet by younger normal-hearing listeners and older listeners with normal-hearing and hearing loss. J Acoust Soc Am 2010; 128: 444-455 DOI: 10.1121/1.3397409.
- 82 Gordon-Salant S, Zion DJ, Espy-Wilson C. Recognition of time-compressed speech does not predict recognition of natural fast-rate speech by older listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 2014; 136: EL268-EL274 DOI: 10.1121/1.4895014.
- 83 Helfer KS, Freyman RL. Aging and Speech-on-Speech Masking. Ear Hear 2008; 29: 87-98 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31815d638b.
- 84 Dubno JR, Dirks DD, Morgan DE. Effects of age and mild hearing loss on speech recognition in noise. J Acoust Soc Am 1984; 76: 87-96 DOI: 10.1121/1.391011.
- 85 Tun PA, Wingfield A. One voice too many: adult age differences in language processing with different types of distracting sounds. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1999; 54: P317-P327 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/54b.5.p317.
- 86 Pronk M, Deeg DJH, Festen JM. et al. Decline in older persons’ ability to recognize speech in noise: the influence of demographic, health-related, environmental, and cognitive factors. Ear Hear 2013; 34: 722-732 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182994eee.
- 87 Füllgrabe C, Moore BCJ, Stone MA. Age-group differences in speech identification despite matched audiometrically normal hearing: contributions from auditory temporal processing and cognition. Front Aging Neurosci 2015; 6: 347 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00347.
- 88 Gallun FJ. Impaired Binaural Hearing in Adults: A Selected Review of the Literature. Front Neurosci 2021; 15: 610957 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2021.610957.
- 89 Hommet C, Mondon K, Berrut G. et al. Central auditory processing in aging: the dichotic listening paradigm. J Nutr Health Aging 2010; 14: 751-756 DOI: 10.1007/s12603-010-0097-7.
- 90 Dillard LK, Fischer ME, Pinto A. et al. Longitudinal Decline on the Dichotic Digits Test. Am J Audiol 2020; 29: 862-872 DOI: 10.1044/2020_AJA-20-00098.
- 91 Harris KC. The Aging Auditory System: Electrophysiology. In: Helfer KS, Bartlett EL, Popper AN, et al., Hrsg. Aging and Hearing: Causes and Consequences. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2020: 117-141
- 92 Morrison C, Rabipour S, Knoefel F. et al. Auditory Event-related Potentials in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Curr Alzheimer Res 2018; 15: 702-715 DOI: 10.2174/1567205015666180123123209.
- 93 Gates GA. Central presbycusis: an emerging view. Otolaryngol--Head Neck Surg Off J Am Acad Otolaryngol-Head Neck Surg 2012; 147: 1-2 DOI: 10.1177/0194599812446282.
- 94 Humes LE, Dubno JR, Gordon-Salant S. et al. Central presbycusis: a review and evaluation of the evidence. J Am Acad Audiol 2012; 23: 635-666 DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.23.8.5.
- 95 Arbeitsgemeinschaft der Wissenschaftlichen Medizinischen Fachgesellschaften (AWMF), Hrsg. S1-Leitlinie 2019 Auditive Verarbeitungs- und Wahrnehmungsstörungen (AVWS) Herausgegeben von der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Phoniatrie und Pädaudiologie
- 96 Schneider BA, Pichora-Fuller K, Daneman M. Effects of Senescent Changes in Audition and Cognition on Spoken Language Comprehension. In: Gordon-Salant S, Frisina RD, Popper AN, et al., Hrsg. The Aging Auditory System. New York, NY: Springer; 2010: 167-210
- 97 Janse E. A non-auditory measure of interference predicts distraction by competing speech in older adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2012; 19: 741-758 DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2011.652590.
- 98 Ward KM, Shen J, Souza PE. et al. Age-Related Differences in Listening Effort During Degraded Speech Recognition. Ear Hear 2017; 38: 74-84 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000355.
- 99 Arlinger S, Lunner T, Lyxell B. et al. The emergence of cognitive hearing science. Scand J Psychol 2009; 50: 371-384 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00753.x.
- 100 Luce PA, Pisoni DB. Recognizing spoken words: the neighborhood activation model. Ear Hear 1998; 19: 1-36 DOI: 10.1097/00003446-199802000-00001.
- 101 Taler V, Aaron GP, Steinmetz LG. et al. Lexical neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition and production in healthy aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2010; 65: 551-560 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbq039.
- 102 Helfer KS, Jesse A. Lexical influences on competing speech perception in younger, middle-aged, and older adults. J Acoust Soc Am 2015; 138: 363-376 DOI: 10.1121/1.4923155.
- 103 Jesse A, Helfer KS. Lexical Influences on Errors in Masked Speech Perception in Younger, Middle-Aged, and Older Adults. J Speech Lang Hear Res JSLHR 2019; 62: 1152-1166 DOI: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-H-ASCC7-18-0091.
- 104 Baddeley A. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol 2012; 63: 1-29 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422.
- 105 Rönnberg J, Holmer E, Rudner M. Cognitive Hearing Science: Three Memory Systems, Two Approaches, and the Ease of Language Understanding Model. J Speech Lang Hear Res JSLHR 2021; 64: 359-370 DOI: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00007.
- 106 Peelle JE. Listening Effort: How the Cognitive Consequences of Acoustic Challenge Are Reflected in Brain and Behavior. Ear Hear 2018; 39: 204-214 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000494.
- 107 Rudner M, Rönnberg J, Lunner T. Working memory supports listening in noise for persons with hearing impairment. J Am Acad Audiol 2011; 22: 156-167 DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.22.3.4.
- 108 Gordon-Salant S, Cole SS. Effects of Age and Working Memory Capacity on Speech Recognition Performance in Noise Among Listeners With Normal Hearing. Ear Hear 2016; 37: 593-602 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000316.
- 109 Benichov J, Cox LC, Tun PA. et al. Word recognition within a linguistic context: effects of age, hearing acuity, verbal ability, and cognitive function. Ear Hear 2012; 33: 250-256 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31822f680f.
- 110 Rogers CS, Jacoby LL, Sommers MS. Frequent false hearing by older adults: the role of age differences in metacognition. Psychol Aging 2012; 27: 33-45 DOI: 10.1037/a0026231.
- 111 Rogers CS. Semantic priming, not repetition priming, is to blame for false hearing. Psychon Bull Rev 2017; 24: 1194-1204 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-016-1185-4.
- 112 Failes E, Sommers MS, Jacoby LL. Blurring past and present: Using false memory to better understand false hearing in young and older adults. Mem Cognit 2020; 48: 1403-1416 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-020-01068-8.
- 113 Van Os M, Kray J, Demberg V. Mishearing as a Side Effect of Rational Language Comprehension in Noise. Front Psychol 2021; 12: 679278 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.679278.
- 114 Pichora-Fuller MK, Kramer SE, Eckert MA. et al. Hearing Impairment and Cognitive Energy: The Framework for Understanding Effortful Listening (FUEL). Ear Hear 2016; 37: 5S DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000312.
- 115 Vos T, Lim SS, Abbafati C. et al. Global burden of 369 diseases and injuries in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. The Lancet 2020; 396: 1204-1222 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30925-9.
- 116 GBD 2019 Dementia Forecasting Collaborators. Estimation of the global prevalence of dementia in 2019 and forecasted prevalence in 2050: an analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Public Health 2022; 7: e105-e125 DOI: 10.1016/S2468-2667(21)00249-8.
- 117 Deutsche Alzheimer Gesellschaft e.V. Infoblatt 1: Die Häufigkeit von Demenzerkrankungen. . Im Internet: https://www.deutsche-alzheimer.de/publikationen/informationsblaetter;
- 118 Wancata J, Musalek M, Alexandrowicz R. et al. Number of dementia sufferers in Europe between the years 2000 and 2050. Eur Psychiatry J Assoc Eur Psychiatr 2003; 18: 306-313 DOI: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2003.03.003.
- 119 Norton S, Matthews FE, Barnes DE. et al. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol 2014; 13: 788-794 DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X.
- 120 Jessen F. Die Nationale Demenzstrategie. Fortschritte Neurol · Psychiatr 2022; 90: 320-325 DOI: 10.1055/a-1808-6459.
- 121 Hans-Holger Bleß, Doron Benjamin Stein Weißbuch Versorgung der frühen Alzheimer Krankheit. Springer; 2021
- 122 Long JM, Holtzman DM. Alzheimer Disease: An Update on Pathobiology and Treatment Strategies. Cell 2019; 179: 312-339 DOI: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.09.001.
- 123 Arbeitsgemeinschaft der Wissenschaftlichen Medizinischen Fachgesellschaften (AWMF), Hrsg. S3-Leitlinie „Demenzen“ (Langversion – Januar 2016)
- 124 Urbach H, Egger K. MRT bei neurodegenerativen Erkrankungen. : 18.
- 125 Crutch SJ, Lehmann M, Schott JM. et al. Posterior cortical atrophy. Lancet Neurol 2012; 11: 170-178 DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70289-7.
- 126 Ossenkoppele R, Pijnenburg YAL, Perry DC. et al. The behavioural/dysexecutive variant of Alzheimer’s disease: clinical, neuroimaging and pathological features. Brain J Neurol 2015; 138: 2732-2749 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awv191.
- 127 Warren JD, Fletcher PD, Golden HL. The paradox of syndromic diversity in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol 2012; 8: 451-464 DOI: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.135.
- 128 Sinha UK, Hollen KM, Rodriguez R. et al. Auditory system degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1993; 43: 779-785 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.43.4.779.
- 129 Goll JC, Kim LG, Hailstone JC. et al. Auditory object cognition in dementia. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49: 2755-2765 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.004.
- 130 Golden HL, Agustus JL, Goll JC. et al. Functional neuroanatomy of auditory scene analysis in Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage Clin 2015; 7: 699-708 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.02.019.
- 131 Golden HL, Agustus JL, Nicholas JM. et al. Functional neuroanatomy of spatial sound processing in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2016; 39: 154-164 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.12.006.
- 132 Goll JC, Kim LG, Ridgway GR. et al. Impairments of auditory scene analysis in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain J Neurol 2012; 135: 190-200 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awr260.
- 133 Idrizbegovic E, Hederstierna C, Dahlquist M. et al. Central auditory function in early Alzheimer’s disease and in mild cognitive impairment. Age Ageing 2011; 40: 249-254 DOI: 10.1093/ageing/afq168.
- 134 Coebergh JAF, McDowell S. van Woerkom TCAM, et al. Auditory Agnosia for Environmental Sounds in Alzheimer’s Disease: Not Hearing and Not Listening?. J Alzheimers Dis JAD 2020; 73: 1407-1419 DOI: 10.3233/JAD-190431.
- 135 Uhlmann RF, Larson EB, Koepsell TD. Hearing impairment and cognitive decline in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. J Am Geriatr Soc 1986; 34: 207-210 DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1986.tb04204.x.
- 136 Lin FR, Metter EJ, O’Brien RJ. et al. Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch Neurol 2011; 68: 214-220 DOI: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.362.
- 137 Taljaard DS, Olaithe M, Brennan-Jones CG. et al. The relationship between hearing impairment and cognitive function: a meta-analysis in adults. Clin Otolaryngol 2016; 41: 718-729 DOI: 10.1111/coa.12607.
- 138 Gates GA, Cobb JL, Linn RT. et al. Central auditory dysfunction, cognitive dysfunction, and dementia in older people. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 1996; 122: 161-167 DOI: 10.1001/archotol.1996.01890140047010.
- 139 Gates GA, Beiser A, Rees TS. et al. Central auditory dysfunction may precede the onset of clinical dementia in people with probable Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 2002; 50: 482-488 DOI: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50114.x.
- 140 Gates GA, Anderson ML, McCurry SM. et al. Central Auditory Dysfunction as a Harbinger of Alzheimer Dementia. Arch Otolaryngol Neck Surg 2011; 137: 390-395 DOI: 10.1001/archoto.2011.28.
- 141 Quaranta N, Coppola F, Casulli M. et al. The prevalence of peripheral and central hearing impairment and its relation to cognition in older adults. Audiol Neurootol 2014; 19: 10-14 DOI: 10.1159/000371597.
- 142 Sardone R, Battista P, Donghia R. et al. Age-Related Central Auditory Processing Disorder, MCI, and Dementia in an Older Population of Southern Italy. Otolaryngol--Head Neck Surg Off J Am Acad Otolaryngol-Head Neck Surg 2020; 163: 348-355 DOI: 10.1177/0194599820913635.
- 143 Mamo SK, Reed NS, Sharrett AR. et al. Relationship Between Domain-Specific Cognitive Function and Speech-in-Noise Performance in Older Adults: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Hearing Pilot Study. Am J Audiol 2019; 28: 1006-1014 DOI: 10.1044/2019_AJA-19-00043.
- 144 Iliadou V, Kaprinis S. Clinical psychoacoustics in Alzheimer’s disease central auditory processing disorders and speech deterioration. Ann Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2003; 2: 12 DOI: 10.1186/1475-2832-2-12.
- 145 Tarawneh HY, Menegola HK, Peou A. et al. Central Auditory Functions of Alzheimer’s Disease and Its Preclinical Stages: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Cells 2022; 11: 1007 DOI: 10.3390/cells11061007.
- 146 Powell DS, Oh ES, Reed NS. et al. Hearing Loss and Cognition: What We Know and Where We Need to Go. Front Aging Neurosci 2022; 13
- 147 Golob EJ, Ringman JM, Irimajiri R. et al. Cortical event-related potentials in preclinical familial Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2009; 73: 1649-1655 DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181c1de77.
- 148 Tönges L, Ehret R, Lorrain M. et al. Epidemiologie der Parkinsonerkrankung und aktuelle ambulante Versorgungskonzepte in Deutschland. Fortschritte Neurol · Psychiatr 2017; 85: 329-335 DOI: 10.1055/s-0043-103275.
- 149 de Lau LML, Breteler MMB. Epidemiology of Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol 2006; 5: 525-535 DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70471-9.
- 150 Heinzel S, Berg D, Binder S. et al. Do We Need to Rethink the Epidemiology and Healthcare Utilization of Parkinson’s Disease in Germany?. Front Neurol 2018; 9: 500 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00500.
- 151 Pringsheim T, Jette N, Frolkis A. et al. The prevalence of Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2014; 29: 1583-1590 DOI: 10.1002/mds.25945.
- 152 Bach J-P, Ziegler U, Deuschl G. et al. Projected numbers of people with movement disorders in the years 2030 and 2050. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2011; 26: 2286-2290 DOI: 10.1002/mds.23878.
- 153 Poewe W, Seppi K, Tanner CM. et al. Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Dis Primer 2017; 3: 17013 DOI: 10.1038/nrdp.2017.13.
- 154 Antony PMA, Diederich NJ, Krüger R. et al. The hallmarks of Parkinson’s disease. FEBS J 2013; 280: 5981-5993 DOI: 10.1111/febs.12335.
- 155 Kalia LV, Lang AE. Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Lond Engl 2015; 386: 896-912 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61393-3.
- 156 Williams-Gray CH, Worth PF. Parkinson’s disease. Medicine (Baltimore) 2016; 44: 542-546 DOI: 10.1016/j.mpmed.2016.06.001.
- 157 Chaudhuri KR, Healy DG, Schapira AHV. et al. Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease: diagnosis and management. Lancet Neurol 2006; 5: 235-245 DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70373-8.
- 158 Riedel O, Klotsche J, Spottke A. et al. Cognitive impairment in 873 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Results from the German Study on Epidemiology of Parkinson’s Disease with Dementia (GEPAD). J Neurol 2008; 255: 255-264 DOI: 10.1007/s00415-008-0720-2.
- 159 Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP. et al. Risk of dementia in Parkinson’s disease: a community-based, prospective study. Neurology 2001; 56: 730-736 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.56.6.730.
- 160 Hobson P, Meara J. Risk and incidence of dementia in a cohort of older subjects with Parkinson’s disease in the United Kingdom. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2004; 19: 1043-1049 DOI: 10.1002/mds.20216.
- 161 Williams-Gray CH, Mason SL, Evans JR. et al. The CamPaIGN study of Parkinson’s disease: 10-year outlook in an incident population-based cohort. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2013; 84: 1258-1264 DOI: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-305277.
- 162 Lai S-W, Liao K-F, Lin C-L. et al. Hearing loss may be a non-motor feature of Parkinson’s disease in older people in Taiwan. Eur J Neurol 2014; 21: 752-757 DOI: 10.1111/ene.12378.
- 163 Vitale C, Marcelli V, Allocca R. et al. Hearing impairment in Parkinson’s disease: expanding the nonmotor phenotype. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2012; 27: 1530-1535 DOI: 10.1002/mds.25149.
- 164 Vitale C, Marcelli V, Abate T. et al. Speech discrimination is impaired in parkinsonian patients: Expanding the audiologic findings of Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2016; 22: S138-S143 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.09.040.
- 165 Jafari Z, Kolb BE, Mohajerani MH. Auditory Dysfunction in Parkinson’s Disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2020; 35: 537-550 DOI: 10.1002/mds.28000.
- 166 Li S, Cheng C, Lu L. et al. Hearing Loss in Neurological Disorders. Front Cell Dev Biol 2021; 9: 716300 DOI: 10.3389/fcell.2021.716300.
- 167 Simonet C, Bestwick J, Jitlal M. et al. Assessment of Risk Factors and Early Presentations of Parkinson Disease in Primary Care in a Diverse UK Population. JAMA Neurol 2022; 79: 359-369 DOI: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2022.0003.
- 168 Yýlmaz S, Karalý E, Tokmak A. et al. Auditory evaluation in Parkinsonian patients. Eur Arch Oto-Rhino-Laryngol Off J Eur Fed Oto-Rhino-Laryngol Soc EUFOS Affil Ger Soc Oto-Rhino-Laryngol – Head Neck Surg 2009; 266: 669-671 DOI: 10.1007/s00405-009-0933-8.
- 169 Shetty K, Krishnan S, Thulaseedharan JV. et al. Asymptomatic Hearing Impairment Frequently Occurs in Early-Onset Parkinson’s Disease. J Mov Disord 2019; 12: 84-90 DOI: 10.14802/jmd.18048.
- 170 Scarpa A, Cassandro C, Vitale C. et al. A comparison of auditory and vestibular dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease and Multiple System Atrophy. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2020; 71: 51-57 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2020.01.018.
- 171 Leme MS, Sanches SGG, Carvallo RMM. Peripheral hearing in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review. Int J Audiol 2022; 1-9 DOI: 10.1080/14992027.2022.2109073.
- 172 Pisani V, Sisto R, Moleti A. et al. An investigation of hearing impairment in de-novo Parkinson’s disease patients: A preliminary study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2015; 21: 987-991 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.06.007.
- 173 Seidel K, Mahlke J, Siswanto S. et al. The brainstem pathologies of Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain Pathol Zurich Switz 2015; 25: 121-135 DOI: 10.1111/bpa.12168.
- 174 Folmer RL, Vachhani JJ, Theodoroff SM. et al. Auditory Processing Abilities of Parkinson’s Disease Patients. BioMed Res Int 2017; 2017: 2618587 DOI: 10.1155/2017/2618587.
- 175 Neel AT. Effects of loud and amplified speech on sentence and word intelligibility in Parkinson disease. J Speech Lang Hear Res JSLHR 2009; 52: 1021-1033 DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/08-0119).
- 176 Sisto R, Viziano A, Stefani A. et al. Lateralization of cochlear dysfunction as a specific biomarker of Parkinson’s disease. Brain Commun 2020; 2: fcaa144 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa144.
- 177 Mollaei F, Shiller DM, Baum SR. et al. The Relationship Between Speech Perceptual Discrimination and Speech Production in Parkinson’s Disease. J Speech Lang Hear Res JSLHR 2019; 62: 4256-4268 DOI: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-18-0425.
- 178 Cochen De Cock V, de Verbizier D, Picot MC. et al. Rhythm disturbances as a potential early marker of Parkinson’s disease in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2020; 7: 280-287 DOI: 10.1002/acn3.50982.
- 179 Shalash AS, Hassan DM, Elrassas HH. et al. Auditory- and Vestibular-Evoked Potentials Correlate with Motor and Non-Motor Features of Parkinson’s Disease. Front Neurol 2017; 8: 55 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00055.
- 180 Liu C, Zhang Y, Tang W. et al. Evoked potential changes in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain Behav 2017; 7: e00703 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.703.
- 181 de Natale ER, Ginatempo F, Paulus KS. et al. Paired neurophysiological and clinical study of the brainstem at different stages of Parkinson’s Disease. Clin Neurophysiol Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol 2015; 126: 1871-1878 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.12.017.
- 182 Pötter-Nerger M, Govender S, Deuschl G. et al. Selective changes of ocular vestibular myogenic potentials in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2015; 30: 584-589 DOI: 10.1002/mds.26114.
- 183 Heitland I, Kenemans JL, Oosting RS. et al. Auditory event-related potentials (P3a, P3b) and genetic variants within the dopamine and serotonin system in healthy females. Behav Brain Res 2013; 249: 55-64 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.013.
- 184 Pfabigan DM, Seidel E-M, Sladky R. et al. P300 amplitude variation is related to ventral striatum BOLD response during gain and loss anticipation: an EEG and fMRI experiment. NeuroImage 2014; 96: 12-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.077.
- 185 Schomaker J, Berendse HW, Foncke EMJ. et al. Novelty processing and memory formation in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2014; 62: 124-136 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.016.
- 186 Solís-Vivanco R, Rodríguez-Violante M, Rodríguez-Agudelo Y. et al. The P3a wave: A reliable neurophysiological measure of Parkinson’s disease duration and severity. Clin Neurophysiol Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol 2015; 126: 2142-2149 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.12.024.
- 187 Solís-Vivanco R, Rodríguez-Violante M, Cervantes-Arriaga A. et al. Brain oscillations reveal impaired novelty detection from early stages of Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage Clin 2018; 18: 923-931 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.03.024.
- 188 Matsui H, Nishinaka K, Oda M. et al. Auditory event-related potentials in Parkinson’s disease: prominent correlation with attention. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2007; 13: 394-398 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2006.12.012.
- 189 Yilmaz FT, Özkaynak SS, Barçin E. Contribution of auditory P300 test to the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Neurol Sci Off J Ital Neurol Soc Ital Soc Clin Neurophysiol 2017; 38: 2103-2109 DOI: 10.1007/s10072-017-3106-3.
- 190 Fan W, Li J, Wei W. et al. Effects of rhythmic auditory stimulation on upper-limb movements in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2022; 101: 27-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2022.06.020.
- 191 Trindade MFD, Viana RA. Effects of auditory or visual stimuli on gait in Parkinsonic patients: a systematic review. Porto Biomed J 2021; 6: e140 DOI: 10.1097/j.pbj.0000000000000140.
- 192 Koshimori Y, Thaut MH. Future perspectives on neural mechanisms underlying rhythm and music based neurorehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease. Ageing Res Rev 2018; 47: 133-139 DOI: 10.1016/j.arr.2018.07.001.
- 193 Slade K, Plack CJ, Nuttall HE. The Effects of Age-Related Hearing Loss on the Brain and Cognitive Function. Trends Neurosci 2020; 43: 810-821 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.07.005.
- 194 Wayne RV, Johnsrude IS. A review of causal mechanisms underlying the link between age-related hearing loss and cognitive decline. Ageing Res Rev 2015; 23: 154-166 DOI: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.06.002.
- 195 Uchida Y, Sugiura S, Nishita Y. et al. Age-related hearing loss and cognitive decline — The potential mechanisms linking the two. Auris Nasus Larynx 2019; 46: 1-9 DOI: 10.1016/j.anl.2018.08.010.
- 196 Oluwole OG, James K, Yalcouye A. et al. Hearing loss and brain disorders: A review of multiple pathologies. Open Med Wars Pol 2022; 17: 61-69 DOI: 10.1515/med-2021-0402.
- 197 Gallacher J, Ilubaera V, Ben-Shlomo Y. et al. Auditory threshold, phonologic demand, and incident dementia. Neurology 2012; 79: 1583-1590 DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31826e263d.
- 198 Deal JA, Betz J, Yaffe K. et al. Hearing Impairment and Incident Dementia and Cognitive Decline in Older Adults: The Health ABC Study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2017; 72: 703-709 DOI: 10.1093/gerona/glw069.
- 199 Dryden A, Allen HA, Henshaw H. et al. The Association Between Cognitive Performance and Speech-in-Noise Perception for Adult Listeners: A Systematic Literature Review and Meta-Analysis. Trends Hear 2017; 21: 2331216517744675 DOI: 10.1177/2331216517744675.
- 200 Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V. et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53: 695-699 DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x.
- 201 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. „Mini-mental state“. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189-198 DOI: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.
- 202 Teng EL, Chui HC. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. J Clin Psychiatry 1987; 48: 314-318
- 203 Jorgensen LE, Palmer CV, Pratt S. et al. The Effect of Decreased Audibility on MMSE Performance: A Measure Commonly Used for Diagnosing Dementia. J Am Acad Audiol 2016; 27: 311-323 DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.15006.
- 204 Dupuis K, Pichora-Fuller MK, Chasteen AL. et al. Effects of hearing and vision impairments on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2015; 22: 413-437 DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2014.968084.
- 205 Wong CG, Rapport LJ, Billings BA. et al. Hearing loss and verbal memory assessment among older adults. Neuropsychology 2019; 33: 47-59 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000489.
- 206 Völter C, Götze L, Bruene-Cohrs U. et al. Hören und Kognition: neurokognitive Testbatterien in der HNO-Heilkunde. HNO 2020; 68: 155-163 DOI: 10.1007/s00106-019-00762-7.
- 207 Speech understanding and aging Working Group on Speech Understanding and Aging. Committee on Hearing, Bioacoustics, and Biomechanics, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Research Council. J Acoust Soc Am 1988; 83: 859-895
- 208 Lindenberger U, Baltes PB. Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: a strong connection. Psychol Aging 1994; 9: 339-355 DOI: 10.1037//0882-7974.9.3.339.
- 209 Kiely KM, Gopinath B, Mitchell P. et al. Cognitive, health, and sociodemographic predictors of longitudinal decline in hearing acuity among older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2012; 67: 997-1003 DOI: 10.1093/gerona/gls066.
- 210 Pichora-Fuller MK. Cognitive aging and auditory information processing. Int J Audiol 2003; 42: 2S26-32S26
- 211 McCoy SL, Tun PA, Cox LC. et al. Hearing loss and perceptual effort: downstream effects on older adults’ memory for speech. Q J Exp Psychol A 2005; 58: 22-33 DOI: 10.1080/02724980443000151.
- 212 Wong PCM, Ettlinger M, Sheppard JP. et al. Neuroanatomical characteristics and speech perception in noise in older adults. Ear Hear 2010; 31: 471-479 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181d709c2.
- 213 Sheppard JP, Wang J-P, Wong PCM. Large-scale cortical functional organization and speech perception across the lifespan. PloS One 2011; 6: e16510 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016510.
- 214 Eckert MA, Vaden KI, Dubno JR. Age-Related Hearing Loss Associations With Changes in Brain Morphology. Trends Hear 2019; 23: 2331216519857267 DOI: 10.1177/2331216519857267.
- 215 Kral A, Sharma A. Developmental neuroplasticity after cochlear implantation. Trends Neurosci 2012; 35: 111-122 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.09.004.
- 216 Kral A. Auditory critical periods: a review from system’s perspective. Neuroscience 2013; 247: 117-133 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.05.021.
- 217 Vernon M. Fifty Years of Research on the Intelligence of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children: A Review of Literature and Discussion of Implications. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2005; 10: 225-231 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/eni024.
- 218 Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev 1996; 103: 403-428 DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403.
- 219 Lipnicki DM, Crawford JD, Dutta R. et al. Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: a collaborative cohort study. PLoS Med 2017; 14: e1002261 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.
- 220 Laughlin GA, McEvoy LK, Barrett-Connor E. et al. Fetuin-A, a new vascular biomarker of cognitive decline in older adults. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 2014; 81: 134-140 DOI: 10.1111/cen.12382.
- 221 Jayakody DMP, Wishart J, Stegeman I. et al. Is There an Association Between Untreated Hearing Loss and Psychosocial Outcomes?. Front Aging Neurosci 2022; 14: 868673 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.868673.
- 222 Pasta A, Szatmari T-I, Christensen JH. et al. Clustering Users Based on Hearing Aid Use: An Exploratory Analysis of Real-World Data. Front Digit Health 2021; 3: 725130 DOI: 10.3389/fdgth.2021.725130.
- 223 Lindenberger U, Ghisletta P. Cognitive and sensory declines in old age: gauging the evidence for a common cause. Psychol Aging 2009; 24: 1-16 DOI: 10.1037/a0014986.
- 224 Deal JA, Goman AM, Albert MS. et al. Hearing treatment for reducing cognitive decline: Design and methods of the Aging and Cognitive Health Evaluation in Elders randomized controlled trial. Alzheimers Dement N Y N 2018; 4: 499-507 DOI: 10.1016/j.trci.2018.08.007.
- 225 Amieva H, Ouvrard C, Giulioli C. et al. Self-Reported Hearing Loss, Hearing Aids, and Cognitive Decline in Elderly Adults: A 25-Year Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2015; 63: 2099-2104 DOI: 10.1111/jgs.13649.
- 226 Ray J, Popli G, Fell G. Association of Cognition and Age-Related Hearing Impairment in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. JAMA Otolaryngol-- Head Neck Surg 2018; 144: 876-882 DOI: 10.1001/jamaoto.2018.1656.
- 227 Maharani A, Dawes P, Nazroo J. et al. Longitudinal Relationship Between Hearing Aid Use and Cognitive Function in Older Americans. J Am Geriatr Soc 2018; 66: 1130-1136 DOI: 10.1111/jgs.15363.
- 228 Sanders ME, Kant E, Smit AL. et al. The effect of hearing aids on cognitive function: A systematic review. PloS One 2021; 16: e0261207 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0261207.
- 229 Dawes P, Cruickshanks KJ, Fischer ME. et al. Hearing-aid use and long-term health outcomes: Hearing handicap, mental health, social engagement, cognitive function, physical health, and mortality. Int J Audiol 2015; 54: 838-844 DOI: 10.3109/14992027.2015.1059503.
- 230 Olze H, Knopke S, Gräbel S. et al. Rapid Positive Influence of Cochlear Implantation on the Quality of Life in Adults 70 Years and Older. Audiol Neurootol 2016; 21: 43-47 DOI: 10.1159/000448354.
- 231 Knopke S, Häussler S, Gräbel S. et al. Age-Dependent Psychological Factors Influencing the Outcome of Cochlear Implantation in Elderly Patients. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2019; 40: e441-e453 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000002179.
- 232 Shin YJ, Fraysse B, Deguine O. et al. Benefits of cochlear implantation in elderly patients. Otolaryngol--Head Neck Surg Off J Am Acad Otolaryngol-Head Neck Surg 2000; 122: 602-606 DOI: 10.1067/mhn.2000.98317.
- 233 Pasanisi E, Bacciu A, Vincenti V. et al. Speech recognition in elderly cochlear implant recipients. Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci 2003; 28: 154-157 DOI: 10.1046/j.1365-2273.2003.00681.x.
- 234 Vermeire K, Brokx JPL, Wuyts FL. et al. Quality-of-life benefit from cochlear implantation in the elderly. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2005; 26: 188-195 DOI: 10.1097/00129492-200503000-00010.
- 235 Moberly AC, Lewis JH, Vasil KJ. et al. Bottom-Up Signal Quality Impacts the Role of Top-Down Cognitive-Linguistic Processing During Speech Recognition by Adults with Cochlear Implants. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2021; 42: S33-S41 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000003377.
- 236 Tao D, Deng R, Jiang Y. et al. Contribution of auditory working memory to speech understanding in mandarin-speaking cochlear implant users. PloS One 2014; 9: e99096 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099096.
- 237 Moberly AC, Houston DM, Harris MS. et al. Verbal working memory and inhibition-concentration in adults with cochlear implants. Laryngoscope Investig Otolaryngol 2017; 2: 254-261 DOI: 10.1002/lio2.90.
- 238 Winn MB. Rapid Release From Listening Effort Resulting From Semantic Context, and Effects of Spectral Degradation and Cochlear Implants. Trends Hear 2016; 20: 2331216516669723 DOI: 10.1177/2331216516669723.
- 239 Mosnier I, Bebear J-P, Marx M. et al. Improvement of cognitive function after cochlear implantation in elderly patients. JAMA Otolaryngol-- Head Neck Surg 2015; 141: 442-450 DOI: 10.1001/jamaoto.2015.129.
- 240 Mosnier I, Vanier A, Bonnard D. et al. Long-Term Cognitive Prognosis of Profoundly Deaf Older Adults After Hearing Rehabilitation Using Cochlear Implants. J Am Geriatr Soc 2018; 66: 1553-1561 DOI: 10.1111/jgs.15445.
- 241 Castiglione A, Benatti A, Velardita C. et al. Aging, Cognitive Decline and Hearing Loss: Effects of Auditory Rehabilitation and Training with Hearing Aids and Cochlear Implants on Cognitive Function and Depression among Older Adults. Audiol Neurootol 2016; 21: 21-28 DOI: 10.1159/000448350.
- 242 Cosetti MK, Pinkston JB, Flores JM. et al. Neurocognitive testing and cochlear implantation: insights into performance in older adults. Clin Interv Aging 2016; 11: 603-613 DOI: 10.2147/CIA.S100255.
- 243 Sonnet M-H, Montaut-Verient B, Niemier J-Y. et al. Cognitive Abilities and Quality of Life After Cochlear Implantation in the Elderly. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2017; 38: e296-e301 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001503.
- 244 Jayakody DMP, Friedland PL, Nel E. et al. Impact of Cochlear Implantation on Cognitive Functions of Older Adults: Pilot Test Results. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2017; 38: e289-e295 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001502.
- 245 Mertens G, Andries E, Claes AJ. et al. Cognitive Improvement After Cochlear Implantation in Older Adults With Severe or Profound Hearing Impairment: A Prospective, Longitudinal, Controlled, Multicenter Study. Ear Hear 2021; 42: 606-614 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000962.
- 246 Völter C, Götze L, Bajewski M. et al. Cognition and Cognitive Reserve in Cochlear Implant Recipients. Front Aging Neurosci 2022; 14: 838214 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.838214.
- 247 Völter C, Götze L, Haubitz I. et al. Impact of Cochlear Implantation on Neurocognitive Subdomains in Adult Cochlear Implant Recipients. Audiol Neurootol 2021; 26: 236-245 DOI: 10.1159/000510855.
- 248 Völter C, Götze L, Dazert S. et al. Can cochlear implantation improve neurocognition in the aging population?. Clin Interv Aging 2018; 13: 701-712 DOI: 10.2147/CIA.S160517.
- 249 Huber M, Roesch S, Pletzer B. et al. Can Cochlear Implantation in Older Adults Reverse Cognitive Decline Due to Hearing Loss?. Ear Hear 2021; 42: 1560-1576 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000001049.
- 250 Knopke S, Schubert A, Häussler SM. et al. Improvement of Working Memory and Processing Speed in Patients over 70 with Bilateral Hearing Impairment Following Unilateral Cochlear Implantation. J Clin Med 2021; 10: 3421 DOI: 10.3390/jcm10153421.
- 251 Sarant J, Harris D, Busby P. et al. The Effect of Cochlear Implants on Cognitive Function in Older Adults: Initial Baseline and 18-Month Follow Up Results for a Prospective International Longitudinal Study. Front Neurosci 2019; 13: 789 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00789.
- 252 Zhan KY, Lewis JH, Vasil KJ. et al. Cognitive Functions in Adults Receiving Cochlear Implants: Predictors of Speech Recognition and Changes After Implantation. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2020; 41: e322-e329 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000002544.
Publication History
Article published online:
02 May 2023
© 2023. The Author(s). This is an open access article published by Thieme under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonDerivative-NonCommercial-License, permitting copying and reproduction so long as the original work is given appropriate credit. Contents may not be used for commercial purposes, or adapted, remixed, transformed or built upon. (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/).
Georg Thieme Verlag KG
Rüdigerstraße 14, 70469 Stuttgart, Germany
-
Literatur
- 1 Stangl, Werner. Online Lexikon für Psychologie und Pädagogik
- 2 Flanagan DP, Dixon SG. The Cattell-Horn-Carroll Theory of Cognitive Abilities. In: Encyclopedia of Special Education. John Wiley & Sons, Ltd; 2014
- 3 American Psychiatric Association, Peter Falkai, Hans-Ulrich Wittchen Diagnostisches und Statistisches Manual Psychischer Störungen DSM-5. 2. korrigierte Auflage 2018. Hogrefe; 2018
- 4 Tucker-Drob EM. Neurocognitive functions and everyday functions change together in old age. Neuropsychology 2011; 25: 368-377 DOI: 10.1037/a0022348.
- 5 Tucker-Drob EM. Cognitive Aging and Dementia: A Life Span Perspective. Annu Rev. Dev Psychol 2019; 1: 177-196 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-devpsych-121318-085204.
- 6 Baltes PB. [Age and aging as incomplete architecture of human ontogenesis]. Z Gerontol Geriatr 1999; 32: 433-448 DOI: 10.1007/s003910050141.
- 7 Tucker-Drob EM, de la Fuente J, Köhncke Y. et al. A strong dependency between changes in fluid and crystallized abilities in human cognitive aging. Sci Adv 2022; 8: eabj2422 DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.abj2422.
- 8 Hartshorne JK, Germine LT. When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychol Sci 2015; 26: 433-443 DOI: 10.1177/0956797614567339.
- 9 Tucker-Drob EM. Global and domain-specific changes in cognition throughout adulthood. Dev Psychol 2011; 47: 331-343 DOI: 10.1037/a0021361.
- 10 Buckner RL. Memory and executive function in aging and AD: multiple factors that cause decline and reserve factors that compensate. Neuron 2004; 44: 195-208 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2004.09.006.
- 11 Hedden T, Gabrieli JDE. Insights into the ageing mind: a view from cognitive neuroscience. Nat Rev Neurosci 2004; 5: 87-96 DOI: 10.1038/nrn1323.
- 12 Jagust W. Vulnerable neural systems and the borderland of brain aging and neurodegeneration. Neuron 2013; 77: 219-234 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.01.002.
- 13 Baltes PB, Dittmann-Kohli F, Kliegl R. Reserve capacity of the elderly in aging-sensitive tests of fluid intelligence: replication and extension. Psychol Aging 1986; 1: 172-177 DOI: 10.1037/0882-7974.1.2.172.
- 14 Stern Y, Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Bartrés-Faz D. et al. Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimers Dement J Alzheimers Assoc 2020; 16: 1305-1311 DOI: 10.1016/j.jalz.2018.07.219.
- 15 Tucker AM, Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in aging. Curr Alzheimer Res 2011; 8: 354-360 DOI: 10.2174/156720511795745320.
- 16 Stenfelt S, Rönnberg J. The signal-cognition interface: interactions between degraded auditory signals and cognitive processes. Scand J Psychol 2009; 50: 385-393 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00748.x.
- 17 Wingfield A, Tun PA. Cognitive Supports and Cognitive Constraints on Comprehension of Spoken Language. J Am Acad Audiol 2007; 18: 548-558 DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.18.7.3.
- 18 Gordon-Salant S, Shader MJ, Wingfield A. Age-Related Changes in Speech Understanding: Peripheral Versus Cognitive Influences. In: Helfer KS, Bartlett EL, Popper AN, et al., Hrsg. Aging and Hearing: Causes and Consequences. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2020: 199-230
- 19 Johnson JCS, Marshall CR, Weil RS. et al. Hearing and dementia: from ears to brain. Brain J Neurol 2021; 144: 391-401 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awaa429.
- 20 World Health Organization World report on hearing. Geneva: World Health Organization; 2021
- 21 Davis A, McMahon CM, Pichora-Fuller KM. et al. Aging and Hearing Health: The Life-course Approach. The Gerontologist 2016; 56: S256-S267 DOI: 10.1093/geront/gnw033.
- 22 Lin FR, Yaffe K, Xia J. et al. Hearing loss and cognitive decline in older adults. JAMA Intern Med 2013; 173: 293-299 DOI: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.1868.
- 23 Loughrey DG, Kelly ME, Kelley GA. et al. Association of Age-Related Hearing Loss With Cognitive Function, Cognitive Impairment, and Dementia: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Otolaryngol-- Head Neck Surg 2018; 144: 115-126 DOI: 10.1001/jamaoto.2017.2513.
- 24 Livingston G, Huntley J, Sommerlad A. et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet Lond Engl 2020; 396: 413-446 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30367-6.
- 25 Livingston G, Sommerlad A, Orgeta V. et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care. Lancet Lond Engl 2017; 390: 2673-2734 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31363-6.
- 26 Rutherford BR, Brewster K, Golub JS. et al. Sensation and Psychiatry: Linking Age-Related Hearing Loss to Late-Life Depression and Cognitive Decline. Am J Psychiatry 2018; 175: 215-224 DOI: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17040423.
- 27 Brewster K, Choi CJ, He X. et al. Hearing Rehabilitative Treatment for Older Adults With Comorbid Hearing Loss and Depression: Effects on Depressive Symptoms and Executive Function. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry Off J Am Assoc Geriatr Psychiatry 2022; 30: 448-458 DOI: 10.1016/j.jagp.2021.08.006.
- 28 Brewster KK, Pavlicova M, Stein A. et al. A pilot randomized controlled trial of hearing aids to improve mood and cognition in older adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 2020; 35: 842-850 DOI: 10.1002/gps.5311.
- 29 Bigelow RT, Reed NS, Brewster KK. et al. Association of Hearing Loss With Psychological Distress and Utilization of Mental Health Services Among Adults in the United States. JAMA Netw Open 2020; 3: e2010986 DOI: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2020.10986.
- 30 Orji A, Kamenov K, Dirac M. et al. Global and regional needs, unmet needs and access to hearing aids. Int J Audiol 2020; 59: 166-172 DOI: 10.1080/14992027.2020.1721577.
- 31 Liberman MC, Kujawa SG. Cochlear synaptopathy in acquired sensorineural hearing loss: Manifestations and mechanisms. Hear Res 2017; 349: 138-147 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2017.01.003.
- 32 Keithley EM. Pathology and mechanisms of cochlear aging. J Neurosci Res 2020; 98: 1674-1684 DOI: 10.1002/jnr.24439.
- 33 Frisina RD, Ding B, Zhu X. et al. Age-related hearing loss: prevention of threshold declines, cell loss and apoptosis in spiral ganglion neurons. Aging 2016; 8: 2081-2099 DOI: 10.18632/aging.101045.
- 34 Kujawa SG, Liberman MC. Synaptopathy in the noise-exposed and aging cochlea: Primary neural degeneration in acquired sensorineural hearing loss. Hear Res 2015; 330: 191-199 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2015.02.009.
- 35 Wu PZ, Liberman LD, Bennett K. et al. Primary Neural Degeneration in the Human Cochlea: Evidence for Hidden Hearing Loss in the Aging Ear. Neuroscience 2019; 407: 8-20 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2018.07.053.
- 36 Gates GA, Mills JH. Presbycusis. The Lancet 2005; 366: 1111-1120 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(05)67423-5.
- 37 Dubno JR, Eckert MA, Lee F-S. et al. Classifying human audiometric phenotypes of age-related hearing loss from animal models. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol JARO 2013; 14: 687-701 DOI: 10.1007/s10162-013-0396-x.
- 38 Fischer N, Weber B, Riechelmann H. [Presbycusis – Age Related Hearing Loss]. Laryngorhinootologie 2016; 95: 497-510 DOI: 10.1055/s-0042-106918.
- 39 Michel O. [DIN EN ISO 7029:2017-06 : The current DIN thresholds for evaluating normal hearing]. HNO 2021; 69: 1014-1018 DOI: 10.1007/s00106-021-01111-3.
- 40 Tremblay KL, Pinto A, Fischer ME. et al. Self-Reported Hearing Difficulties Among Adults With Normal Audiograms: The Beaver Dam Offspring Study. Ear Hear 2015; 36: e290-e299 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000195.
- 41 Schaette R, McAlpine D. Tinnitus with a normal audiogram: physiological evidence for hidden hearing loss and computational model. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 2011; 31: 13452-13457 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2156-11.2011.
- 42 Bajin MD, Dahm V, Lin VYW. Hidden hearing loss: current concepts. Curr Opin Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2022; DOI: 10.1097/MOO.0000000000000824.
- 43 C Kohrman D, Wan G, Cassinotti L et al. Hidden Hearing Loss: A Disorder with Multiple Etiologies and Mechanisms. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Med 2020; 10: a035493 DOI: 10.1101/cshperspect.a035493.
- 44 Pienkowski M. On the Etiology of Listening Difficulties in Noise Despite Clinically Normal Audiograms. Ear Hear 2017; 38: 135-148 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000388.
- 45 Plack CJ, Barker D, Prendergast G. Perceptual consequences of „hidden“ hearing loss. Trends Hear 2014; 18: 2331216514550621 DOI: 10.1177/2331216514550621.
- 46 Parthasarathy A, Kujawa SG. Synaptopathy in the Aging Cochlea: Characterizing Early-Neural Deficits in Auditory Temporal Envelope Processing. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 2018; 38: 7108-7119 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3240-17.2018.
- 47 Wan G, Corfas G. Transient auditory nerve demyelination as a new mechanism for hidden hearing loss. Nat Commun 2017; 8: 14487 DOI: 10.1038/ncomms14487.
- 48 Choi JE, Seok JM, Ahn J. et al. Hidden hearing loss in patients with Charcot-Marie-Tooth disease type 1A. Sci Rep 2018; 8: 10335 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-018-28501-y.
- 49 Mulders WHAM, Chin IL, Robertson D. Persistent hair cell malfunction contributes to hidden hearing loss. Hear Res 2018; 361: 45-51 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2018.02.001.
- 50 Hoben R, Easow G, Pevzner S. et al. Outer Hair Cell and Auditory Nerve Function in Speech Recognition in Quiet and in Background Noise. Front Neurosci 2017; 11: 157 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00157.
- 51 Sergeyenko Y, Lall K, Liberman MC. et al. Age-related cochlear synaptopathy: an early-onset contributor to auditory functional decline. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 2013; 33: 13686-13694 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1783-13.2013.
- 52 Grant KJ, Mepani AM, Wu P. et al. Electrophysiological markers of cochlear function correlate with hearing-in-noise performance among audiometrically normal subjects. J Neurophysiol 2020; 124: 418-431 DOI: 10.1152/jn.00016.2020.
- 53 Jayakody DMP, Friedland PL, Martins RN. et al. Impact of Aging on the Auditory System and Related Cognitive Functions: A Narrative Review. Front Neurosci 2018; 12: 125 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00125.
- 54 Ouda L, Profant O, Syka J. Age-related changes in the central auditory system. Cell Tissue Res 2015; 361: 337-358 DOI: 10.1007/s00441-014-2107-2.
- 55 Hedman AM, van Haren NEM, Schnack HG. et al. Human brain changes across the life span: a review of 56 longitudinal magnetic resonance imaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 2012; 33: 1987-2002 DOI: 10.1002/hbm.21334.
- 56 Mori S, Onda K, Fujita S. et al. Brain atrophy in middle age using magnetic resonance imaging scans from Japan’s health screening programme. Brain Commun 2022; 4: fcac211 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcac211.
- 57 Miller KL, Alfaro-Almagro F, Bangerter NK. et al. Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat Neurosci 2016; 19: 1523-1536 DOI: 10.1038/nn.4393.
- 58 Lemaitre H, Goldman AL, Sambataro F. et al. Normal age-related brain morphometric changes: nonuniformity across cortical thickness, surface area and gray matter volume?. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33: e1-e9 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.07.013.
- 59 Raz N, Gunning FM, Head D. et al. Selective aging of the human cerebral cortex observed in vivo: differential vulnerability of the prefrontal gray matter. Cereb Cortex N Y N 1991 1997; 7: 268-282 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/7.3.268.
- 60 Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Head D. et al. Differential aging of the medial temporal lobe: a study of a five-year change. Neurology 2004; 62: 433-438 DOI: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000106466.09835.46.
- 61 Raz N, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM. et al. Vascular health and longitudinal changes in brain and cognition in middle-aged and older adults. Neuropsychology 2007; 21: 149-157 DOI: 10.1037/0894-4105.21.2.149.
- 62 Westlye LT, Walhovd KB, Dale AM. et al. Life-span changes of the human brain white matter: diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) and volumetry. Cereb Cortex N Y N 1991 2010; 20: 2055-2068 DOI: 10.1093/cercor/bhp280.
- 63 Vidal-Pineiro D, Parker N, Shin J. et al. Cellular correlates of cortical thinning throughout the lifespan. Sci Rep 2020; 10: 21803 DOI: 10.1038/s41598-020-78471-3.
- 64 Scahill RI, Frost C, Jenkins R. et al. A longitudinal study of brain volume changes in normal aging using serial registered magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Neurol 2003; 60: 989-994 DOI: 10.1001/archneur.60.7.989.
- 65 Braak H, Thal DR, Ghebremedhin E. et al. Stages of the pathologic process in Alzheimer disease: age categories from 1 to 100 years. J Neuropathol Exp Neurol 2011; 70: 960-969 DOI: 10.1097/NEN.0b013e318232a379.
- 66 Pettemeridou E, Kallousia E, Constantinidou F. Regional Brain Volume, Brain Reserve and MMSE Performance in Healthy Aging From the NEUROAGE Cohort: Contributions of Sex, Education, and Depression Symptoms. Front Aging Neurosci 2021; 13: 711301 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.711301.
- 67 Kalpouzos G, Persson J, Nyberg L. Local brain atrophy accounts for functional activity differences in normal aging. Neurobiol Aging 2012; 33: 623.e1-623.e13 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.02.021.
- 68 Lin FR, Ferrucci L, An Y. et al. Association of hearing impairment with brain volume changes in older adults. NeuroImage 2014; 90: 84-92 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.12.059.
- 69 Husain FT, Medina RE, Davis CW. et al. Neuroanatomical changes due to hearing loss and chronic tinnitus: a combined VBM and DTI study. Brain Res 2011; 1369: 74-88 DOI: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.10.095.
- 70 Boyen K, Langers DRM, de Kleine E. et al. Gray matter in the brain: differences associated with tinnitus and hearing loss. Hear Res 2013; 295: 67-78 DOI: 10.1016/j.heares.2012.02.010.
- 71 Rosemann S, Thiel CM. Neuroanatomical changes associated with age-related hearing loss and listening effort. Brain Struct Funct 2020; 225: 2689-2700 DOI: 10.1007/s00429-020-02148-w.
- 72 Peelle JE, Troiani V, Grossman M. et al. Hearing loss in older adults affects neural systems supporting speech comprehension. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci 2011; 31: 12638-12643 DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2559-11.2011.
- 73 Eckert MA, Cute SL, Vaden KI. et al. Auditory cortex signs of age-related hearing loss. J Assoc Res Otolaryngol JARO 2012; 13: 703-713 DOI: 10.1007/s10162-012-0332-5.
- 74 Chang Y, Lee S-H, Lee Y-J. et al. Auditory neural pathway evaluation on sensorineural hearing loss using diffusion tensor imaging. NeuroReport 2004; 15: 1699-1703 DOI: 10.1097/01.wnr.0000134584.10207.1a.
- 75 Profant O, Balogová Z, Dezortová M. et al. Metabolic changes in the auditory cortex in presbycusis demonstrated by MR spectroscopy. Exp Gerontol 2013; 48: 795-800 DOI: 10.1016/j.exger.2013.04.012.
- 76 Gao F, Wang G, Ma W. et al. Decreased auditory GABA+concentrations in presbycusis demonstrated by edited magnetic resonance spectroscopy. NeuroImage 2015; 106: 311-316 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.11.023.
- 77 Peelle JE, Wingfield A. The Neural Consequences of Age-Related Hearing Loss. Trends Neurosci 2016; 39: 486-497 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2016.05.001.
- 78 Gordon-Salant S, Yeni-Komshian G, Fitzgibbons P. The role of temporal cues in word identification by younger and older adults: effects of sentence context. J Acoust Soc Am 2008; 124: 3249-3260 DOI: 10.1121/1.2982409.
- 79 Schvartz KC, Chatterjee M, Gordon-Salant S. Recognition of spectrally degraded phonemes by younger, middle-aged, and older normal-hearing listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 2008; 124: 3972-3988 DOI: 10.1121/1.2997434.
- 80 Goupell MJ, Gaskins CR, Shader MJ. et al. Age-Related Differences in the Processing of Temporal Envelope and Spectral Cues in a Speech Segment. Ear Hear 2017; 38: e335-e342 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000447.
- 81 Gordon-Salant S, Yeni-Komshian GH, Fitzgibbons PJ. Recognition of accented English in quiet by younger normal-hearing listeners and older listeners with normal-hearing and hearing loss. J Acoust Soc Am 2010; 128: 444-455 DOI: 10.1121/1.3397409.
- 82 Gordon-Salant S, Zion DJ, Espy-Wilson C. Recognition of time-compressed speech does not predict recognition of natural fast-rate speech by older listeners. J Acoust Soc Am 2014; 136: EL268-EL274 DOI: 10.1121/1.4895014.
- 83 Helfer KS, Freyman RL. Aging and Speech-on-Speech Masking. Ear Hear 2008; 29: 87-98 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31815d638b.
- 84 Dubno JR, Dirks DD, Morgan DE. Effects of age and mild hearing loss on speech recognition in noise. J Acoust Soc Am 1984; 76: 87-96 DOI: 10.1121/1.391011.
- 85 Tun PA, Wingfield A. One voice too many: adult age differences in language processing with different types of distracting sounds. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 1999; 54: P317-P327 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/54b.5.p317.
- 86 Pronk M, Deeg DJH, Festen JM. et al. Decline in older persons’ ability to recognize speech in noise: the influence of demographic, health-related, environmental, and cognitive factors. Ear Hear 2013; 34: 722-732 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3182994eee.
- 87 Füllgrabe C, Moore BCJ, Stone MA. Age-group differences in speech identification despite matched audiometrically normal hearing: contributions from auditory temporal processing and cognition. Front Aging Neurosci 2015; 6: 347 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00347.
- 88 Gallun FJ. Impaired Binaural Hearing in Adults: A Selected Review of the Literature. Front Neurosci 2021; 15: 610957 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2021.610957.
- 89 Hommet C, Mondon K, Berrut G. et al. Central auditory processing in aging: the dichotic listening paradigm. J Nutr Health Aging 2010; 14: 751-756 DOI: 10.1007/s12603-010-0097-7.
- 90 Dillard LK, Fischer ME, Pinto A. et al. Longitudinal Decline on the Dichotic Digits Test. Am J Audiol 2020; 29: 862-872 DOI: 10.1044/2020_AJA-20-00098.
- 91 Harris KC. The Aging Auditory System: Electrophysiology. In: Helfer KS, Bartlett EL, Popper AN, et al., Hrsg. Aging and Hearing: Causes and Consequences. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2020: 117-141
- 92 Morrison C, Rabipour S, Knoefel F. et al. Auditory Event-related Potentials in Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer’s Disease. Curr Alzheimer Res 2018; 15: 702-715 DOI: 10.2174/1567205015666180123123209.
- 93 Gates GA. Central presbycusis: an emerging view. Otolaryngol--Head Neck Surg Off J Am Acad Otolaryngol-Head Neck Surg 2012; 147: 1-2 DOI: 10.1177/0194599812446282.
- 94 Humes LE, Dubno JR, Gordon-Salant S. et al. Central presbycusis: a review and evaluation of the evidence. J Am Acad Audiol 2012; 23: 635-666 DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.23.8.5.
- 95 Arbeitsgemeinschaft der Wissenschaftlichen Medizinischen Fachgesellschaften (AWMF), Hrsg. S1-Leitlinie 2019 Auditive Verarbeitungs- und Wahrnehmungsstörungen (AVWS) Herausgegeben von der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Phoniatrie und Pädaudiologie
- 96 Schneider BA, Pichora-Fuller K, Daneman M. Effects of Senescent Changes in Audition and Cognition on Spoken Language Comprehension. In: Gordon-Salant S, Frisina RD, Popper AN, et al., Hrsg. The Aging Auditory System. New York, NY: Springer; 2010: 167-210
- 97 Janse E. A non-auditory measure of interference predicts distraction by competing speech in older adults. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2012; 19: 741-758 DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2011.652590.
- 98 Ward KM, Shen J, Souza PE. et al. Age-Related Differences in Listening Effort During Degraded Speech Recognition. Ear Hear 2017; 38: 74-84 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000355.
- 99 Arlinger S, Lunner T, Lyxell B. et al. The emergence of cognitive hearing science. Scand J Psychol 2009; 50: 371-384 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-9450.2009.00753.x.
- 100 Luce PA, Pisoni DB. Recognizing spoken words: the neighborhood activation model. Ear Hear 1998; 19: 1-36 DOI: 10.1097/00003446-199802000-00001.
- 101 Taler V, Aaron GP, Steinmetz LG. et al. Lexical neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition and production in healthy aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 2010; 65: 551-560 DOI: 10.1093/geronb/gbq039.
- 102 Helfer KS, Jesse A. Lexical influences on competing speech perception in younger, middle-aged, and older adults. J Acoust Soc Am 2015; 138: 363-376 DOI: 10.1121/1.4923155.
- 103 Jesse A, Helfer KS. Lexical Influences on Errors in Masked Speech Perception in Younger, Middle-Aged, and Older Adults. J Speech Lang Hear Res JSLHR 2019; 62: 1152-1166 DOI: 10.1044/2018_JSLHR-H-ASCC7-18-0091.
- 104 Baddeley A. Working memory: theories, models, and controversies. Annu Rev Psychol 2012; 63: 1-29 DOI: 10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100422.
- 105 Rönnberg J, Holmer E, Rudner M. Cognitive Hearing Science: Three Memory Systems, Two Approaches, and the Ease of Language Understanding Model. J Speech Lang Hear Res JSLHR 2021; 64: 359-370 DOI: 10.1044/2020_JSLHR-20-00007.
- 106 Peelle JE. Listening Effort: How the Cognitive Consequences of Acoustic Challenge Are Reflected in Brain and Behavior. Ear Hear 2018; 39: 204-214 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000494.
- 107 Rudner M, Rönnberg J, Lunner T. Working memory supports listening in noise for persons with hearing impairment. J Am Acad Audiol 2011; 22: 156-167 DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.22.3.4.
- 108 Gordon-Salant S, Cole SS. Effects of Age and Working Memory Capacity on Speech Recognition Performance in Noise Among Listeners With Normal Hearing. Ear Hear 2016; 37: 593-602 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000316.
- 109 Benichov J, Cox LC, Tun PA. et al. Word recognition within a linguistic context: effects of age, hearing acuity, verbal ability, and cognitive function. Ear Hear 2012; 33: 250-256 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e31822f680f.
- 110 Rogers CS, Jacoby LL, Sommers MS. Frequent false hearing by older adults: the role of age differences in metacognition. Psychol Aging 2012; 27: 33-45 DOI: 10.1037/a0026231.
- 111 Rogers CS. Semantic priming, not repetition priming, is to blame for false hearing. Psychon Bull Rev 2017; 24: 1194-1204 DOI: 10.3758/s13423-016-1185-4.
- 112 Failes E, Sommers MS, Jacoby LL. Blurring past and present: Using false memory to better understand false hearing in young and older adults. Mem Cognit 2020; 48: 1403-1416 DOI: 10.3758/s13421-020-01068-8.
- 113 Van Os M, Kray J, Demberg V. Mishearing as a Side Effect of Rational Language Comprehension in Noise. Front Psychol 2021; 12: 679278 DOI: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.679278.
- 114 Pichora-Fuller MK, Kramer SE, Eckert MA. et al. Hearing Impairment and Cognitive Energy: The Framework for Understanding Effortful Listening (FUEL). Ear Hear 2016; 37: 5S DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000312.
- 115 Vos T, Lim SS, Abbafati C. et al. Global burden of 369 diseases and injuries in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. The Lancet 2020; 396: 1204-1222 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30925-9.
- 116 GBD 2019 Dementia Forecasting Collaborators. Estimation of the global prevalence of dementia in 2019 and forecasted prevalence in 2050: an analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Public Health 2022; 7: e105-e125 DOI: 10.1016/S2468-2667(21)00249-8.
- 117 Deutsche Alzheimer Gesellschaft e.V. Infoblatt 1: Die Häufigkeit von Demenzerkrankungen. . Im Internet: https://www.deutsche-alzheimer.de/publikationen/informationsblaetter;
- 118 Wancata J, Musalek M, Alexandrowicz R. et al. Number of dementia sufferers in Europe between the years 2000 and 2050. Eur Psychiatry J Assoc Eur Psychiatr 2003; 18: 306-313 DOI: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2003.03.003.
- 119 Norton S, Matthews FE, Barnes DE. et al. Potential for primary prevention of Alzheimer’s disease: an analysis of population-based data. Lancet Neurol 2014; 13: 788-794 DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(14)70136-X.
- 120 Jessen F. Die Nationale Demenzstrategie. Fortschritte Neurol · Psychiatr 2022; 90: 320-325 DOI: 10.1055/a-1808-6459.
- 121 Hans-Holger Bleß, Doron Benjamin Stein Weißbuch Versorgung der frühen Alzheimer Krankheit. Springer; 2021
- 122 Long JM, Holtzman DM. Alzheimer Disease: An Update on Pathobiology and Treatment Strategies. Cell 2019; 179: 312-339 DOI: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.09.001.
- 123 Arbeitsgemeinschaft der Wissenschaftlichen Medizinischen Fachgesellschaften (AWMF), Hrsg. S3-Leitlinie „Demenzen“ (Langversion – Januar 2016)
- 124 Urbach H, Egger K. MRT bei neurodegenerativen Erkrankungen. : 18.
- 125 Crutch SJ, Lehmann M, Schott JM. et al. Posterior cortical atrophy. Lancet Neurol 2012; 11: 170-178 DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(11)70289-7.
- 126 Ossenkoppele R, Pijnenburg YAL, Perry DC. et al. The behavioural/dysexecutive variant of Alzheimer’s disease: clinical, neuroimaging and pathological features. Brain J Neurol 2015; 138: 2732-2749 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awv191.
- 127 Warren JD, Fletcher PD, Golden HL. The paradox of syndromic diversity in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol 2012; 8: 451-464 DOI: 10.1038/nrneurol.2012.135.
- 128 Sinha UK, Hollen KM, Rodriguez R. et al. Auditory system degeneration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1993; 43: 779-785 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.43.4.779.
- 129 Goll JC, Kim LG, Hailstone JC. et al. Auditory object cognition in dementia. Neuropsychologia 2011; 49: 2755-2765 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.06.004.
- 130 Golden HL, Agustus JL, Goll JC. et al. Functional neuroanatomy of auditory scene analysis in Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroImage Clin 2015; 7: 699-708 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.02.019.
- 131 Golden HL, Agustus JL, Nicholas JM. et al. Functional neuroanatomy of spatial sound processing in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 2016; 39: 154-164 DOI: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2015.12.006.
- 132 Goll JC, Kim LG, Ridgway GR. et al. Impairments of auditory scene analysis in Alzheimer’s disease. Brain J Neurol 2012; 135: 190-200 DOI: 10.1093/brain/awr260.
- 133 Idrizbegovic E, Hederstierna C, Dahlquist M. et al. Central auditory function in early Alzheimer’s disease and in mild cognitive impairment. Age Ageing 2011; 40: 249-254 DOI: 10.1093/ageing/afq168.
- 134 Coebergh JAF, McDowell S. van Woerkom TCAM, et al. Auditory Agnosia for Environmental Sounds in Alzheimer’s Disease: Not Hearing and Not Listening?. J Alzheimers Dis JAD 2020; 73: 1407-1419 DOI: 10.3233/JAD-190431.
- 135 Uhlmann RF, Larson EB, Koepsell TD. Hearing impairment and cognitive decline in senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type. J Am Geriatr Soc 1986; 34: 207-210 DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1986.tb04204.x.
- 136 Lin FR, Metter EJ, O’Brien RJ. et al. Hearing loss and incident dementia. Arch Neurol 2011; 68: 214-220 DOI: 10.1001/archneurol.2010.362.
- 137 Taljaard DS, Olaithe M, Brennan-Jones CG. et al. The relationship between hearing impairment and cognitive function: a meta-analysis in adults. Clin Otolaryngol 2016; 41: 718-729 DOI: 10.1111/coa.12607.
- 138 Gates GA, Cobb JL, Linn RT. et al. Central auditory dysfunction, cognitive dysfunction, and dementia in older people. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 1996; 122: 161-167 DOI: 10.1001/archotol.1996.01890140047010.
- 139 Gates GA, Beiser A, Rees TS. et al. Central auditory dysfunction may precede the onset of clinical dementia in people with probable Alzheimer’s disease. J Am Geriatr Soc 2002; 50: 482-488 DOI: 10.1046/j.1532-5415.2002.50114.x.
- 140 Gates GA, Anderson ML, McCurry SM. et al. Central Auditory Dysfunction as a Harbinger of Alzheimer Dementia. Arch Otolaryngol Neck Surg 2011; 137: 390-395 DOI: 10.1001/archoto.2011.28.
- 141 Quaranta N, Coppola F, Casulli M. et al. The prevalence of peripheral and central hearing impairment and its relation to cognition in older adults. Audiol Neurootol 2014; 19: 10-14 DOI: 10.1159/000371597.
- 142 Sardone R, Battista P, Donghia R. et al. Age-Related Central Auditory Processing Disorder, MCI, and Dementia in an Older Population of Southern Italy. Otolaryngol--Head Neck Surg Off J Am Acad Otolaryngol-Head Neck Surg 2020; 163: 348-355 DOI: 10.1177/0194599820913635.
- 143 Mamo SK, Reed NS, Sharrett AR. et al. Relationship Between Domain-Specific Cognitive Function and Speech-in-Noise Performance in Older Adults: The Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities Hearing Pilot Study. Am J Audiol 2019; 28: 1006-1014 DOI: 10.1044/2019_AJA-19-00043.
- 144 Iliadou V, Kaprinis S. Clinical psychoacoustics in Alzheimer’s disease central auditory processing disorders and speech deterioration. Ann Gen Hosp Psychiatry 2003; 2: 12 DOI: 10.1186/1475-2832-2-12.
- 145 Tarawneh HY, Menegola HK, Peou A. et al. Central Auditory Functions of Alzheimer’s Disease and Its Preclinical Stages: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. Cells 2022; 11: 1007 DOI: 10.3390/cells11061007.
- 146 Powell DS, Oh ES, Reed NS. et al. Hearing Loss and Cognition: What We Know and Where We Need to Go. Front Aging Neurosci 2022; 13
- 147 Golob EJ, Ringman JM, Irimajiri R. et al. Cortical event-related potentials in preclinical familial Alzheimer disease. Neurology 2009; 73: 1649-1655 DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181c1de77.
- 148 Tönges L, Ehret R, Lorrain M. et al. Epidemiologie der Parkinsonerkrankung und aktuelle ambulante Versorgungskonzepte in Deutschland. Fortschritte Neurol · Psychiatr 2017; 85: 329-335 DOI: 10.1055/s-0043-103275.
- 149 de Lau LML, Breteler MMB. Epidemiology of Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Neurol 2006; 5: 525-535 DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70471-9.
- 150 Heinzel S, Berg D, Binder S. et al. Do We Need to Rethink the Epidemiology and Healthcare Utilization of Parkinson’s Disease in Germany?. Front Neurol 2018; 9: 500 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00500.
- 151 Pringsheim T, Jette N, Frolkis A. et al. The prevalence of Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2014; 29: 1583-1590 DOI: 10.1002/mds.25945.
- 152 Bach J-P, Ziegler U, Deuschl G. et al. Projected numbers of people with movement disorders in the years 2030 and 2050. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2011; 26: 2286-2290 DOI: 10.1002/mds.23878.
- 153 Poewe W, Seppi K, Tanner CM. et al. Parkinson disease. Nat Rev Dis Primer 2017; 3: 17013 DOI: 10.1038/nrdp.2017.13.
- 154 Antony PMA, Diederich NJ, Krüger R. et al. The hallmarks of Parkinson’s disease. FEBS J 2013; 280: 5981-5993 DOI: 10.1111/febs.12335.
- 155 Kalia LV, Lang AE. Parkinson’s disease. Lancet Lond Engl 2015; 386: 896-912 DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61393-3.
- 156 Williams-Gray CH, Worth PF. Parkinson’s disease. Medicine (Baltimore) 2016; 44: 542-546 DOI: 10.1016/j.mpmed.2016.06.001.
- 157 Chaudhuri KR, Healy DG, Schapira AHV. et al. Non-motor symptoms of Parkinson’s disease: diagnosis and management. Lancet Neurol 2006; 5: 235-245 DOI: 10.1016/S1474-4422(06)70373-8.
- 158 Riedel O, Klotsche J, Spottke A. et al. Cognitive impairment in 873 patients with idiopathic Parkinson’s disease. Results from the German Study on Epidemiology of Parkinson’s Disease with Dementia (GEPAD). J Neurol 2008; 255: 255-264 DOI: 10.1007/s00415-008-0720-2.
- 159 Aarsland D, Andersen K, Larsen JP. et al. Risk of dementia in Parkinson’s disease: a community-based, prospective study. Neurology 2001; 56: 730-736 DOI: 10.1212/wnl.56.6.730.
- 160 Hobson P, Meara J. Risk and incidence of dementia in a cohort of older subjects with Parkinson’s disease in the United Kingdom. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2004; 19: 1043-1049 DOI: 10.1002/mds.20216.
- 161 Williams-Gray CH, Mason SL, Evans JR. et al. The CamPaIGN study of Parkinson’s disease: 10-year outlook in an incident population-based cohort. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2013; 84: 1258-1264 DOI: 10.1136/jnnp-2013-305277.
- 162 Lai S-W, Liao K-F, Lin C-L. et al. Hearing loss may be a non-motor feature of Parkinson’s disease in older people in Taiwan. Eur J Neurol 2014; 21: 752-757 DOI: 10.1111/ene.12378.
- 163 Vitale C, Marcelli V, Allocca R. et al. Hearing impairment in Parkinson’s disease: expanding the nonmotor phenotype. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2012; 27: 1530-1535 DOI: 10.1002/mds.25149.
- 164 Vitale C, Marcelli V, Abate T. et al. Speech discrimination is impaired in parkinsonian patients: Expanding the audiologic findings of Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2016; 22: S138-S143 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.09.040.
- 165 Jafari Z, Kolb BE, Mohajerani MH. Auditory Dysfunction in Parkinson’s Disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2020; 35: 537-550 DOI: 10.1002/mds.28000.
- 166 Li S, Cheng C, Lu L. et al. Hearing Loss in Neurological Disorders. Front Cell Dev Biol 2021; 9: 716300 DOI: 10.3389/fcell.2021.716300.
- 167 Simonet C, Bestwick J, Jitlal M. et al. Assessment of Risk Factors and Early Presentations of Parkinson Disease in Primary Care in a Diverse UK Population. JAMA Neurol 2022; 79: 359-369 DOI: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2022.0003.
- 168 Yýlmaz S, Karalý E, Tokmak A. et al. Auditory evaluation in Parkinsonian patients. Eur Arch Oto-Rhino-Laryngol Off J Eur Fed Oto-Rhino-Laryngol Soc EUFOS Affil Ger Soc Oto-Rhino-Laryngol – Head Neck Surg 2009; 266: 669-671 DOI: 10.1007/s00405-009-0933-8.
- 169 Shetty K, Krishnan S, Thulaseedharan JV. et al. Asymptomatic Hearing Impairment Frequently Occurs in Early-Onset Parkinson’s Disease. J Mov Disord 2019; 12: 84-90 DOI: 10.14802/jmd.18048.
- 170 Scarpa A, Cassandro C, Vitale C. et al. A comparison of auditory and vestibular dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease and Multiple System Atrophy. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2020; 71: 51-57 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2020.01.018.
- 171 Leme MS, Sanches SGG, Carvallo RMM. Peripheral hearing in Parkinson’s disease: a systematic review. Int J Audiol 2022; 1-9 DOI: 10.1080/14992027.2022.2109073.
- 172 Pisani V, Sisto R, Moleti A. et al. An investigation of hearing impairment in de-novo Parkinson’s disease patients: A preliminary study. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2015; 21: 987-991 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2015.06.007.
- 173 Seidel K, Mahlke J, Siswanto S. et al. The brainstem pathologies of Parkinson’s disease and dementia with Lewy bodies. Brain Pathol Zurich Switz 2015; 25: 121-135 DOI: 10.1111/bpa.12168.
- 174 Folmer RL, Vachhani JJ, Theodoroff SM. et al. Auditory Processing Abilities of Parkinson’s Disease Patients. BioMed Res Int 2017; 2017: 2618587 DOI: 10.1155/2017/2618587.
- 175 Neel AT. Effects of loud and amplified speech on sentence and word intelligibility in Parkinson disease. J Speech Lang Hear Res JSLHR 2009; 52: 1021-1033 DOI: 10.1044/1092-4388(2008/08-0119).
- 176 Sisto R, Viziano A, Stefani A. et al. Lateralization of cochlear dysfunction as a specific biomarker of Parkinson’s disease. Brain Commun 2020; 2: fcaa144 DOI: 10.1093/braincomms/fcaa144.
- 177 Mollaei F, Shiller DM, Baum SR. et al. The Relationship Between Speech Perceptual Discrimination and Speech Production in Parkinson’s Disease. J Speech Lang Hear Res JSLHR 2019; 62: 4256-4268 DOI: 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-S-18-0425.
- 178 Cochen De Cock V, de Verbizier D, Picot MC. et al. Rhythm disturbances as a potential early marker of Parkinson’s disease in idiopathic REM sleep behavior disorder. Ann Clin Transl Neurol 2020; 7: 280-287 DOI: 10.1002/acn3.50982.
- 179 Shalash AS, Hassan DM, Elrassas HH. et al. Auditory- and Vestibular-Evoked Potentials Correlate with Motor and Non-Motor Features of Parkinson’s Disease. Front Neurol 2017; 8: 55 DOI: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00055.
- 180 Liu C, Zhang Y, Tang W. et al. Evoked potential changes in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Brain Behav 2017; 7: e00703 DOI: 10.1002/brb3.703.
- 181 de Natale ER, Ginatempo F, Paulus KS. et al. Paired neurophysiological and clinical study of the brainstem at different stages of Parkinson’s Disease. Clin Neurophysiol Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol 2015; 126: 1871-1878 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.12.017.
- 182 Pötter-Nerger M, Govender S, Deuschl G. et al. Selective changes of ocular vestibular myogenic potentials in Parkinson’s disease. Mov Disord Off J Mov Disord Soc 2015; 30: 584-589 DOI: 10.1002/mds.26114.
- 183 Heitland I, Kenemans JL, Oosting RS. et al. Auditory event-related potentials (P3a, P3b) and genetic variants within the dopamine and serotonin system in healthy females. Behav Brain Res 2013; 249: 55-64 DOI: 10.1016/j.bbr.2013.04.013.
- 184 Pfabigan DM, Seidel E-M, Sladky R. et al. P300 amplitude variation is related to ventral striatum BOLD response during gain and loss anticipation: an EEG and fMRI experiment. NeuroImage 2014; 96: 12-21 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.03.077.
- 185 Schomaker J, Berendse HW, Foncke EMJ. et al. Novelty processing and memory formation in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2014; 62: 124-136 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.07.016.
- 186 Solís-Vivanco R, Rodríguez-Violante M, Rodríguez-Agudelo Y. et al. The P3a wave: A reliable neurophysiological measure of Parkinson’s disease duration and severity. Clin Neurophysiol Off J Int Fed Clin Neurophysiol 2015; 126: 2142-2149 DOI: 10.1016/j.clinph.2014.12.024.
- 187 Solís-Vivanco R, Rodríguez-Violante M, Cervantes-Arriaga A. et al. Brain oscillations reveal impaired novelty detection from early stages of Parkinson’s disease. NeuroImage Clin 2018; 18: 923-931 DOI: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.03.024.
- 188 Matsui H, Nishinaka K, Oda M. et al. Auditory event-related potentials in Parkinson’s disease: prominent correlation with attention. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2007; 13: 394-398 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2006.12.012.
- 189 Yilmaz FT, Özkaynak SS, Barçin E. Contribution of auditory P300 test to the diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment in Parkinson’s disease. Neurol Sci Off J Ital Neurol Soc Ital Soc Clin Neurophysiol 2017; 38: 2103-2109 DOI: 10.1007/s10072-017-3106-3.
- 190 Fan W, Li J, Wei W. et al. Effects of rhythmic auditory stimulation on upper-limb movements in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat Disord 2022; 101: 27-30 DOI: 10.1016/j.parkreldis.2022.06.020.
- 191 Trindade MFD, Viana RA. Effects of auditory or visual stimuli on gait in Parkinsonic patients: a systematic review. Porto Biomed J 2021; 6: e140 DOI: 10.1097/j.pbj.0000000000000140.
- 192 Koshimori Y, Thaut MH. Future perspectives on neural mechanisms underlying rhythm and music based neurorehabilitation in Parkinson’s disease. Ageing Res Rev 2018; 47: 133-139 DOI: 10.1016/j.arr.2018.07.001.
- 193 Slade K, Plack CJ, Nuttall HE. The Effects of Age-Related Hearing Loss on the Brain and Cognitive Function. Trends Neurosci 2020; 43: 810-821 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.07.005.
- 194 Wayne RV, Johnsrude IS. A review of causal mechanisms underlying the link between age-related hearing loss and cognitive decline. Ageing Res Rev 2015; 23: 154-166 DOI: 10.1016/j.arr.2015.06.002.
- 195 Uchida Y, Sugiura S, Nishita Y. et al. Age-related hearing loss and cognitive decline — The potential mechanisms linking the two. Auris Nasus Larynx 2019; 46: 1-9 DOI: 10.1016/j.anl.2018.08.010.
- 196 Oluwole OG, James K, Yalcouye A. et al. Hearing loss and brain disorders: A review of multiple pathologies. Open Med Wars Pol 2022; 17: 61-69 DOI: 10.1515/med-2021-0402.
- 197 Gallacher J, Ilubaera V, Ben-Shlomo Y. et al. Auditory threshold, phonologic demand, and incident dementia. Neurology 2012; 79: 1583-1590 DOI: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e31826e263d.
- 198 Deal JA, Betz J, Yaffe K. et al. Hearing Impairment and Incident Dementia and Cognitive Decline in Older Adults: The Health ABC Study. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2017; 72: 703-709 DOI: 10.1093/gerona/glw069.
- 199 Dryden A, Allen HA, Henshaw H. et al. The Association Between Cognitive Performance and Speech-in-Noise Perception for Adult Listeners: A Systematic Literature Review and Meta-Analysis. Trends Hear 2017; 21: 2331216517744675 DOI: 10.1177/2331216517744675.
- 200 Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V. et al. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc 2005; 53: 695-699 DOI: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x.
- 201 Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. „Mini-mental state“. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975; 12: 189-198 DOI: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6.
- 202 Teng EL, Chui HC. The Modified Mini-Mental State (3MS) examination. J Clin Psychiatry 1987; 48: 314-318
- 203 Jorgensen LE, Palmer CV, Pratt S. et al. The Effect of Decreased Audibility on MMSE Performance: A Measure Commonly Used for Diagnosing Dementia. J Am Acad Audiol 2016; 27: 311-323 DOI: 10.3766/jaaa.15006.
- 204 Dupuis K, Pichora-Fuller MK, Chasteen AL. et al. Effects of hearing and vision impairments on the Montreal Cognitive Assessment. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 2015; 22: 413-437 DOI: 10.1080/13825585.2014.968084.
- 205 Wong CG, Rapport LJ, Billings BA. et al. Hearing loss and verbal memory assessment among older adults. Neuropsychology 2019; 33: 47-59 DOI: 10.1037/neu0000489.
- 206 Völter C, Götze L, Bruene-Cohrs U. et al. Hören und Kognition: neurokognitive Testbatterien in der HNO-Heilkunde. HNO 2020; 68: 155-163 DOI: 10.1007/s00106-019-00762-7.
- 207 Speech understanding and aging Working Group on Speech Understanding and Aging. Committee on Hearing, Bioacoustics, and Biomechanics, Commission on Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education, National Research Council. J Acoust Soc Am 1988; 83: 859-895
- 208 Lindenberger U, Baltes PB. Sensory functioning and intelligence in old age: a strong connection. Psychol Aging 1994; 9: 339-355 DOI: 10.1037//0882-7974.9.3.339.
- 209 Kiely KM, Gopinath B, Mitchell P. et al. Cognitive, health, and sociodemographic predictors of longitudinal decline in hearing acuity among older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 2012; 67: 997-1003 DOI: 10.1093/gerona/gls066.
- 210 Pichora-Fuller MK. Cognitive aging and auditory information processing. Int J Audiol 2003; 42: 2S26-32S26
- 211 McCoy SL, Tun PA, Cox LC. et al. Hearing loss and perceptual effort: downstream effects on older adults’ memory for speech. Q J Exp Psychol A 2005; 58: 22-33 DOI: 10.1080/02724980443000151.
- 212 Wong PCM, Ettlinger M, Sheppard JP. et al. Neuroanatomical characteristics and speech perception in noise in older adults. Ear Hear 2010; 31: 471-479 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0b013e3181d709c2.
- 213 Sheppard JP, Wang J-P, Wong PCM. Large-scale cortical functional organization and speech perception across the lifespan. PloS One 2011; 6: e16510 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0016510.
- 214 Eckert MA, Vaden KI, Dubno JR. Age-Related Hearing Loss Associations With Changes in Brain Morphology. Trends Hear 2019; 23: 2331216519857267 DOI: 10.1177/2331216519857267.
- 215 Kral A, Sharma A. Developmental neuroplasticity after cochlear implantation. Trends Neurosci 2012; 35: 111-122 DOI: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.09.004.
- 216 Kral A. Auditory critical periods: a review from system’s perspective. Neuroscience 2013; 247: 117-133 DOI: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.05.021.
- 217 Vernon M. Fifty Years of Research on the Intelligence of Deaf and Hard-of-Hearing Children: A Review of Literature and Discussion of Implications. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ 2005; 10: 225-231 DOI: 10.1093/deafed/eni024.
- 218 Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev 1996; 103: 403-428 DOI: 10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.403.
- 219 Lipnicki DM, Crawford JD, Dutta R. et al. Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: a collaborative cohort study. PLoS Med 2017; 14: e1002261 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.
- 220 Laughlin GA, McEvoy LK, Barrett-Connor E. et al. Fetuin-A, a new vascular biomarker of cognitive decline in older adults. Clin Endocrinol (Oxf) 2014; 81: 134-140 DOI: 10.1111/cen.12382.
- 221 Jayakody DMP, Wishart J, Stegeman I. et al. Is There an Association Between Untreated Hearing Loss and Psychosocial Outcomes?. Front Aging Neurosci 2022; 14: 868673 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.868673.
- 222 Pasta A, Szatmari T-I, Christensen JH. et al. Clustering Users Based on Hearing Aid Use: An Exploratory Analysis of Real-World Data. Front Digit Health 2021; 3: 725130 DOI: 10.3389/fdgth.2021.725130.
- 223 Lindenberger U, Ghisletta P. Cognitive and sensory declines in old age: gauging the evidence for a common cause. Psychol Aging 2009; 24: 1-16 DOI: 10.1037/a0014986.
- 224 Deal JA, Goman AM, Albert MS. et al. Hearing treatment for reducing cognitive decline: Design and methods of the Aging and Cognitive Health Evaluation in Elders randomized controlled trial. Alzheimers Dement N Y N 2018; 4: 499-507 DOI: 10.1016/j.trci.2018.08.007.
- 225 Amieva H, Ouvrard C, Giulioli C. et al. Self-Reported Hearing Loss, Hearing Aids, and Cognitive Decline in Elderly Adults: A 25-Year Study. J Am Geriatr Soc 2015; 63: 2099-2104 DOI: 10.1111/jgs.13649.
- 226 Ray J, Popli G, Fell G. Association of Cognition and Age-Related Hearing Impairment in the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. JAMA Otolaryngol-- Head Neck Surg 2018; 144: 876-882 DOI: 10.1001/jamaoto.2018.1656.
- 227 Maharani A, Dawes P, Nazroo J. et al. Longitudinal Relationship Between Hearing Aid Use and Cognitive Function in Older Americans. J Am Geriatr Soc 2018; 66: 1130-1136 DOI: 10.1111/jgs.15363.
- 228 Sanders ME, Kant E, Smit AL. et al. The effect of hearing aids on cognitive function: A systematic review. PloS One 2021; 16: e0261207 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0261207.
- 229 Dawes P, Cruickshanks KJ, Fischer ME. et al. Hearing-aid use and long-term health outcomes: Hearing handicap, mental health, social engagement, cognitive function, physical health, and mortality. Int J Audiol 2015; 54: 838-844 DOI: 10.3109/14992027.2015.1059503.
- 230 Olze H, Knopke S, Gräbel S. et al. Rapid Positive Influence of Cochlear Implantation on the Quality of Life in Adults 70 Years and Older. Audiol Neurootol 2016; 21: 43-47 DOI: 10.1159/000448354.
- 231 Knopke S, Häussler S, Gräbel S. et al. Age-Dependent Psychological Factors Influencing the Outcome of Cochlear Implantation in Elderly Patients. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2019; 40: e441-e453 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000002179.
- 232 Shin YJ, Fraysse B, Deguine O. et al. Benefits of cochlear implantation in elderly patients. Otolaryngol--Head Neck Surg Off J Am Acad Otolaryngol-Head Neck Surg 2000; 122: 602-606 DOI: 10.1067/mhn.2000.98317.
- 233 Pasanisi E, Bacciu A, Vincenti V. et al. Speech recognition in elderly cochlear implant recipients. Clin Otolaryngol Allied Sci 2003; 28: 154-157 DOI: 10.1046/j.1365-2273.2003.00681.x.
- 234 Vermeire K, Brokx JPL, Wuyts FL. et al. Quality-of-life benefit from cochlear implantation in the elderly. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2005; 26: 188-195 DOI: 10.1097/00129492-200503000-00010.
- 235 Moberly AC, Lewis JH, Vasil KJ. et al. Bottom-Up Signal Quality Impacts the Role of Top-Down Cognitive-Linguistic Processing During Speech Recognition by Adults with Cochlear Implants. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2021; 42: S33-S41 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000003377.
- 236 Tao D, Deng R, Jiang Y. et al. Contribution of auditory working memory to speech understanding in mandarin-speaking cochlear implant users. PloS One 2014; 9: e99096 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0099096.
- 237 Moberly AC, Houston DM, Harris MS. et al. Verbal working memory and inhibition-concentration in adults with cochlear implants. Laryngoscope Investig Otolaryngol 2017; 2: 254-261 DOI: 10.1002/lio2.90.
- 238 Winn MB. Rapid Release From Listening Effort Resulting From Semantic Context, and Effects of Spectral Degradation and Cochlear Implants. Trends Hear 2016; 20: 2331216516669723 DOI: 10.1177/2331216516669723.
- 239 Mosnier I, Bebear J-P, Marx M. et al. Improvement of cognitive function after cochlear implantation in elderly patients. JAMA Otolaryngol-- Head Neck Surg 2015; 141: 442-450 DOI: 10.1001/jamaoto.2015.129.
- 240 Mosnier I, Vanier A, Bonnard D. et al. Long-Term Cognitive Prognosis of Profoundly Deaf Older Adults After Hearing Rehabilitation Using Cochlear Implants. J Am Geriatr Soc 2018; 66: 1553-1561 DOI: 10.1111/jgs.15445.
- 241 Castiglione A, Benatti A, Velardita C. et al. Aging, Cognitive Decline and Hearing Loss: Effects of Auditory Rehabilitation and Training with Hearing Aids and Cochlear Implants on Cognitive Function and Depression among Older Adults. Audiol Neurootol 2016; 21: 21-28 DOI: 10.1159/000448350.
- 242 Cosetti MK, Pinkston JB, Flores JM. et al. Neurocognitive testing and cochlear implantation: insights into performance in older adults. Clin Interv Aging 2016; 11: 603-613 DOI: 10.2147/CIA.S100255.
- 243 Sonnet M-H, Montaut-Verient B, Niemier J-Y. et al. Cognitive Abilities and Quality of Life After Cochlear Implantation in the Elderly. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2017; 38: e296-e301 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001503.
- 244 Jayakody DMP, Friedland PL, Nel E. et al. Impact of Cochlear Implantation on Cognitive Functions of Older Adults: Pilot Test Results. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2017; 38: e289-e295 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000001502.
- 245 Mertens G, Andries E, Claes AJ. et al. Cognitive Improvement After Cochlear Implantation in Older Adults With Severe or Profound Hearing Impairment: A Prospective, Longitudinal, Controlled, Multicenter Study. Ear Hear 2021; 42: 606-614 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000962.
- 246 Völter C, Götze L, Bajewski M. et al. Cognition and Cognitive Reserve in Cochlear Implant Recipients. Front Aging Neurosci 2022; 14: 838214 DOI: 10.3389/fnagi.2022.838214.
- 247 Völter C, Götze L, Haubitz I. et al. Impact of Cochlear Implantation on Neurocognitive Subdomains in Adult Cochlear Implant Recipients. Audiol Neurootol 2021; 26: 236-245 DOI: 10.1159/000510855.
- 248 Völter C, Götze L, Dazert S. et al. Can cochlear implantation improve neurocognition in the aging population?. Clin Interv Aging 2018; 13: 701-712 DOI: 10.2147/CIA.S160517.
- 249 Huber M, Roesch S, Pletzer B. et al. Can Cochlear Implantation in Older Adults Reverse Cognitive Decline Due to Hearing Loss?. Ear Hear 2021; 42: 1560-1576 DOI: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000001049.
- 250 Knopke S, Schubert A, Häussler SM. et al. Improvement of Working Memory and Processing Speed in Patients over 70 with Bilateral Hearing Impairment Following Unilateral Cochlear Implantation. J Clin Med 2021; 10: 3421 DOI: 10.3390/jcm10153421.
- 251 Sarant J, Harris D, Busby P. et al. The Effect of Cochlear Implants on Cognitive Function in Older Adults: Initial Baseline and 18-Month Follow Up Results for a Prospective International Longitudinal Study. Front Neurosci 2019; 13: 789 DOI: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00789.
- 252 Zhan KY, Lewis JH, Vasil KJ. et al. Cognitive Functions in Adults Receiving Cochlear Implants: Predictors of Speech Recognition and Changes After Implantation. Otol Neurotol Off Publ Am Otol Soc Am Neurotol Soc Eur Acad Otol Neurotol 2020; 41: e322-e329 DOI: 10.1097/MAO.0000000000002544.